Historial Background to Positive Psychology in North America

Humanistic Psychology

The story of positive psychology (PP) in America begins long prior to the modern movement called PP, and the humanistic psychologists provide a reasonable place to begin. Humanist psychologists were the first psychologists who focused on the positive side of people—their innate goodness and natural tendency towards the self-actualization of their potentials. Carl Rogers and Abraham Maslow were the leading figures in this movement.

The humanistic approach began as a reaction against the determinism of psychoanalysis and behaviorism, both of which were dominant forces in psychology in the 1950s and 60s. Humanistic psychology advocated the need to study the whole person and the subjective perception and phenomenological experience of individuals. From this holistic perspective, behavior cannot be fully understood simply by objective observation apart from the subjective meaning of individuals and their intersubjective verification. Humanistic psychologists maintain that, ultimately, people are motivated by their beliefs and perceptions of the world and innate needs more than by their circumstances.

It is interesting that despite Seligman and Csikszentmihalyi’s (2000) disparaging remark about humanistic psychology, it remains an influential aspect of PP (Froh, 2004). In fact, more than half of the positive psychologists covered in this chapter have been influenced by humanistic psychology. It may be argued that a PP inspired and influenced by humanistic psychology has more depth and enduring influence than a PP without a rich humanistic heritage.

For humanistic psychologists (Maslow, 1954; Rogers, 1961/1995), the responsible use of freedom is essential to become fully functional human beings. They argued that people can achieve their vision of the good life only when they can responsibly exercise their freedom to choose their own authentic path and develop their full potential to achieve their life goals. Thus, their true happiness is a by-product of self-determination and self-actualization.

This kind of happiness is known as “eudaimonic” happiness, as it does not depend merely on feeling good or living a comfortable and pleasurable life as in the case of “hedonic” happiness; rather, it results from an intentional free choice to live an authentic and meaningful life. Thus, in spite of PP’s attempt to distance itself from humanistic psychology, the humanistic thinking regarding freedom, self-determination, peak experience, and eudaimonia can be found in Csikszentmihalyi’s (1990) model of flow, Emmons’ (2005) research on spiritual goal striving, Ryff’s (1989) model of psychological well-being, Ryan and Deci’s (2000) self-determination theory, Wong’s (2011c) second wave of PP, and Huta’ (2016a) research on eudaimonia.

Positive Psychology 1.0 in the U.S.A.

In the United States, much of the credit for the enormous success of the contemporary PP community of researchers deserves to be attributed to Martin Seligman. He said that his main mission during his 1998 presidency of the American Psychological Association (APA) was to create a science of PP (Seligman, 1999a). Before that presidency, he had already started marketing some of his ideas in popular books on learned optimism (1990) and on optimism in children (1995), but it was during his presidency that he championed PP as a new science. He shared his vision, in part, via his presidential columns in the APA Monitor (e.g., Seligman, 1998), which went out to all the members of the APA. After his presidency ended, he continued to recruit others to the field through strategies including invitation-only gatherings for elite young researchers and also through the provision of grants to scholars interested in the field.

The initial topics that Seligman proposed for PP—positive states, positive traits, and positive institutions (Seligman & Csikszentmihalyi, 2000)—are indeed worthy topics, and a number of American scholars were already working on these. It is not so much Seligman’s original ideas as his ability to attract the cream of the crop in mainstream psychology that accounts for the success of PP 1.0. For example, Robert Emmons, who started studying well-being with Ed Diener (Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985) years before Seligman’s presidency, became the founding editor of The Journal of Positive Psychology in 2006. Barbara Fredrickson, who published important articles on positive emotion in 1998 (Fredrickson, 1998; Fredrickson & Levenson, 1998), subsequently received the first Templeton Prize in Positive Psychology in 2000. Also, Csikszentmihalyi, who was already renowned for his work on flow (1990), joined in with the movement along with many others. Perhaps it would be more accurate to describe the PP movement not as a new science, but instead as a new research community that attracted not only some of the best-known researchers, but also many psychologists who became enamored with the potential of this movement.

The field of PP has had its share of detractors, including Lazarus (2003), who attracted much attention with criticisms such as his assertion that positive psychologists “attack the psychology of the past to create the illusion that what they offer is new and different” (p. 107). James Coyne has also criticized PP and particularly the evidence base for some PP interventions not only in peer-reviewed work (Coyne, Tennen, & Ranchor, 2010), but also through his influential PLOS blog (e.g., Coyne, 2014a, b). For a comprehensive critique of PP, see Wong and Roy (in press). On a more popular level, Barbara Ehrenreich (2010) argued that pressuring people to be happy can do more harm than good, and her claims are supported by recent research, such as Oettingen’s findings that some forms of positive thinking predict maladaptive consequences (for a review, see Oettingen, 2014).

In spite of the detractors, the field advanced in a number of ways and continued to gather adherents and institutional support. For example, a U.S. based organization called the International Positive Psychology Association (IPPA) was formed in 2007, and the organization’s first Congress occurred in 2009 in Philadelphia. There is now a Ph.D. program in PP (e.g., Claremont Graduate School) and many students also take a Ph.D. in PP topics in a typical graduate program in psychology (e.g., social, clinical). Master’s level training in PP has also proliferated in North America, but some education programs in PP put diminished emphasis on broad knowledge of psychology and psychological research methods when compared to most typical graduate programs in psychology. The Masters of Applied Positive Psychology (MAPP) program at the University of Pennsylvania, for example, is a liberal arts program, and it does not require any specific undergraduate psychology courses as prerequisites. The curriculum does include one research methods course, but it does not require any coursework in other foundational areas of psychology. In spite of its lack of psychology training, many MAPP graduates market themselves as experts in the science of happiness and well-being. This situation is even more concerning due to the proliferation of PP post-graduate certificate programs. These programs are typically offered by positive psychologists or MAPP graduates to train coaches and consultants and promise to provide a potentially lucrative career for certificate providers and graduates. Though these programs contribute in some ways to Miller’s (1969) campaign to give psychology away so that the public can use it, they also produce some graduates with minimal scientific skills who may overstate the power of positive psychological interventions, thus turning PP into a pseudoscience.

Along with this growth in PP, the field has become closely associated with business. Some business schools are beginning to integrate PP into their coursework. Claremont Graduate School even offers a Ph.D. in positive organizational psychology. Also, for most PP associations, including the IPPA, both the membership and conference programs reflect a mixture of academics, business coaches, and consultants. This inclusion suggests that many people in the business community have bought into the vision of PP. One negative aspect of this inclusion is that the scientific basis of PP is becoming diluted with premature applications and over-generalizations at the workplace (Wong, Ivtzan, & Lomas, 2016).

The flourishing of Martin Seligman’s PP since 1998 can be attributed in part to the confluence of big money, big science, and big names (Wong, 2011a). Seligman’s strategy of recruiting leading psychologists to his PP network and his ability in communicating with and appealing to the business community and consumer society are major reasons for the near-instant popularity of PP; many positive psychologists and coaches have profited from marketing the science of happiness (Wong, Ivtzan, & Lomas, 2016; Wong & Roy, in press).

In some ways, Seligman’s message of positivity also resonates with the same crowd that was once attracted to humanistic psychology. It may also be argued that there has always been a trait of optimism and positive thinking in the American psyche (Ehrenreich, 2010). In spite of the rich heritage of humanistic-existential psychology, Seligman decided to distance his PP movement (PP 1.0) from this heritage (Seligman & Csikszentmihalyi, 2000). This decision has rippling effects that eventually required a course correction in the second wave of PP (PP 2.0; Wong, 2011c). Because the PP 1.0 championed by Seligman fails to integrate the rich humanistic tradition, the emerging movement of PP 2.0 represents an attempt to reclaim PP (Wong, 2011b) and integrate it with humanistic-existential psychology (Wong, 2009a, 2011c). What separates PP 2.0 from PP 1.0 is not just the recognition of positive potentials in negative emotions, but the emphasis on humanistic values and the dark side of human existence.

Positive Psychology in Canada

Given American PP’s ethnocentric biases and Seligman’s penchant to “colonize” other countries and cultures with his brand of PP as representing the universal, Wong’s (2011c) PP 2.0 is a reaction against such excesses. From his Asian upbringing, Wong noticed that American PP is full of “yang”—its focus on optimism, the bright side of life, and the strengths to actualize and achieve important goals; in contrast, Chinese PP is full of “yin”—its keen awareness of the tragedies of life, and its focus on endurance, flexibility, humility, acceptance, and the ideal of being like water. His PP 2.0 represents an integration between East and West, through the dialectical principles of yin and yang. Hopefully, this yin-yang model may serve as a global framework to describe PP in different countries.

By virtue of its proximity to the United States and a common heritage, Canada has always struggled to maintain its unique cultural identity as a mosaic society. This is also the case with respect to PP. In spite of the dominant influence of American PP, Canada is the home of existential PP (Wong, 2009a) and PP 2.0 (Wong, 2011c). Drawing upon European existential thoughts—especially Viktor Frankl’s logotherapy—and Chinese dialectical philosophy (Wong, 2009b, 2016a), Wong developed the unique brand of Canadian PP and promoted it through the Biennial International Meaning Conferences based in Canada.

PP 2.0 is inherently cross-cultural, existential, and much more complex than the initial tenets of positive emotion, positive traits, and positive institutions. The starting point of PP 2.0 is embracing the dark side of human existence as the proper context for research and understanding human flourishing and well-being, just as the medical science starts by accepting the reality that we live in a world full of bacteria and viruses. The aim of PP 2.0 is to bring out the best in individuals and society in spite of inevitable human suffering and evil. Secondly, PP 2.0 favors the dialectical principle of yin and yang as a more realistic way to approach negative and positive human experiences than the binary or dichotomous way.

Consequently, PP 2.0 does not confine itself to neutral or positive territories; it covers the totality of human experiences and is relevant to under-privileged and suffering people. Finally, PP 2.0 emphasizes the importance of the construct validity of variables pertaining to the good life based on both the existential and cross-cultural literature in order to achieve both internal and external validity. In sum, the PP of Canada reflects the mosaic culture of Canada, particularly with its European and Asian influences.

On the global stage of PP, two competing forces are often at work in various countries. Canada provides a good example of the clashing of two competing visions. On the one hand, there is the dominating force of PP from America, with all its big names, big money, influential publications, and evidence-based PP interventions, which may not always be culturally appropriate. An important element in this mix is the training of foreign students through the MAPP program previously described. Often, graduates from MAPP promote their coaching business with science as their calling card, and establish a local Positive Psychology Association as their guild; in other words, they serve as “colonizers” of American PP (for a critique, see Wong & Roy, in press). On the other hand, there are indigenous positive psychologists trained in their own country and shaped by their own culture. They struggle to establish their own indigenous identity by developing culturally sensitive research programs and interventions often with two handicaps—without the necessary funding and under the huge shadow of American PP.

The dominant force of marketing PP to the business world and general public through a much-hyped and watered-down version of American PP research and the home-grown force of developing an indigenous PP have not found a way to work together to advance PP and benefit their country. The difficulty of merging these two forces was clearly evident in the early days of the Canadian Positive Psychology Association (Wong, 2013a).

Major Positive Psychologists of North America: Theory, Research, Assessment, and Practice

Abraham H. Maslow (1908-1970)

The story of PP in the United States and Canada involves many individuals, and a significant portion of this chapter will review a few individuals who have contributed to PP in unique and significant ways. Abraham Maslow is included in this chapter not only because he first coined the term “positive psychology” in his 1954 book Motivation and Personality, but also because of his profound impact on PP. His is most known for his theory of the hierarchy of needs, which has been influential in psychology, management, sociology, and psychiatry.

Maslow (1961) rejected European existentialism and advocated the need to “push toward the establishment of another branch of psychology, the psychology of the fully evolved and authentic self and its ways of being” (p. 56). He coined the term the “Third Force” to identify this new branch of psychology.

Maslow is perhaps best known for his theory of the hierarchy of needs, which consists of five levels: (a) physiological needs, (b) safety and security needs, (c) the need for love and belonging, (d) esteem needs, and (e) the need for self-actualization. The first four needs are deficient needs or “D-motives” because people are motivated to fill the deficiency in these needs. Self-actualization motives represent growth-oriented needs at the “being” level; therefore, they are called the “B-values” or “B-motives.” Wong (2005) pointed out that the hierarchy model does not mean a rigid stage model, in which one needs to complete one level in order to move to the next level; in fact, these needs can be met simultaneously or in a reverse manner. For example, some people may value their calling as more important than their personal safety. The bottom line is that Maslow (1954) believed that the unifying motivational principle is to pursue higher needs. This quote summarizes his positive motivational theory of personality:

Human life will never be understood unless its highest aspirations are taken into account. Growth, self-actualization, the striving toward health, the quest for identity and autonomy, the yearning for excellence (and other ways of phrasing the striving “upward”) must by now be accepted beyond question as a widespread and perhaps universal human tendency. (Maslow, 1954, xii-xiii)

A close reading of Maslow (1943) does not support the simplistic interpretation that higher needs fail to emerge until lower needs are fully met, so even people with difficult lives can experience strong drives at the top end of the hierarchy, and this fact has relevance to therapy and other interventions with people in need.

In his old age, Maslow was puzzled by two questions: First, he recognized that some of his self-actualized friends were “prima donnas” who were self-centered and could not work together. Second, he wondered what motivated people who have already actualized. His solution to these problems was to add self-transcendence as the last or the highest stage of human development. Thus, in order to become fully functioning human beings, self-actualizers need to continue to be driven by meta-motivation and devote their lives to helping others actualize themselves. He also expanded the list of B-motives, which now includes the following: truth, goodness, beauty, wholeness, aliveness, uniqueness, perfection, completion, justice, simplicity, richness, effortlessness, playfulness, and self-sufficiency. Maslow died before he could fully develop his revised needs hierarchy. Koltko-Rivera (2006) introduced Maslow’s highest human motivational need to a broader psychology audience. Maslow’s book, The Farther Reaches of Human Nature (1971) was published posthumously.

Maslow’s theory of human motivation emphasized the inner push towards self-actualization, self-transcendence, and the peak experience. Maslow (1954) described the peak experience as a “tremendous intensification of any of the experiences in which there is loss of self or transcendence of [self]” (p. 165; emphasis original); it includes a sense of rapture, wonder, and ecstasy, which can transform people’s view of themselves and the world around them. Maslow’s main contribution was his emphasis on the positive existential givens and the human potential for the transcendental, spiritual level of human existence. His theory remains one of the most complete and influential theories of PP, a theory that is based on his noble and optimistic view of human nature and human potentials.

Maslow was opposed to the atomistic, dichotomist thinking that is still prevalent in PP. Much like William James’s (1912) “radical empiricism,” Maslow (1970) believed that it was possible to integrate experiential subjectivity with experimental objectivity; he wanted to “integrate the healthily animal, material, and selfish with the naturalistically transcendent, spiritual, and axiological” (p. 5). Throughout his research career, Maslow attempted to humanize and trans-humanize the non-personal science so that it can account for the unique and holistic human experiences (Maslow, 1966/2002).

Mihaly Csikszentmihalyi

Another person who made significant contributions to PP before Seligman’s founding of the field was Csikszentmihalyi. Csikszentmihalyi (2014) experienced significant hardship in his early life, including living as a stateless poverty-stricken refugee in Italy after the Soviet takeover of Hungary and his father’s resignation from an ambassadorial position. Csikszentmihalyi had also lost two brothers and a number of other loved ones to the fight against the Soviets in Hungary.

Amidst his difficulties, Csikszentmihalyi (2014) seems to have found some solace in the life of the mind. In his early teens, he read works by Carl Jung and also acquired familiarity with the works of Pierre Teilhard de Chardin, a French Jesuit who took an optimistic view of humanity’s progress. He reported that, at age 15, he had already dreamt of escaping his difficult position in life by becoming a writer.

In 1956, he moved to Chicago to study psychology, but found that his interests in Jung and Teilhard were not shared by his fellow students or the instructors (Csikszentmihalyi, 2014). It seems that his peers were less interested in the bigger life questions raised by these writers and more focused on smaller questions that could be more easily addressed through empirical research.

As his career continued, Csikszentmihalyi demonstrated an ability to bring concepts and perspectives from other areas of study into psychology. His writings often include references to philosophers, historical figures, and religious traditions. In fact, one of his early publications explored the sociological implications of the thought of Teilhard (Csikszentmihalyi, 1970). His career path after leaving Italy may have contributed to his openness to ideas from a broader array of disciplines. For his Ph.D., he worked within the Committee on Human Development at the University of Chicago, a group that to this day is noted for multi-disciplinary perspectives on academic topics. Furthermore, his first academic job was not in psychology, but instead in sociology and anthropology. Still later in his career, he moved to the management department at Claremont Graduate University, further demonstrating his comfort working with people and ideas from other disciplines.

One of Csikszentmihalyi’s greatest contributions to PP has been his concept of flow, which he outlined long before the emergence of PP 1.0. He wrote about this concept in both academic publications (e.g., 1975) and also in his popular book on the topic (1990). His early descriptions of flow varied somewhat, but twenty-five years after his first publication, Csikszentmihalyi (2000) described flow as a “state of optimal experience that people report when they are intensely involved in doing something that is fun to do” (p. 381).

Two aspects of this theory of flow are particularly notable for PP. First, PP has gone astray, in some ways, by not heeding some of the realizations Csikszentmihalyi developed as part of his studies of flow. In particular, he noticed that lasting pleasure and growth fail to emerge from seeking purely short term happiness. Csikszentmihalyi has been differentiated from many other positive psychologists by his explicit repudiation of purely hedonistic definitions of happiness. In contrast, he argued that enjoyment can often emerge from circumstances that are not pleasant and are even painful, such as climbing a mountain during a snow storm, perfecting a difficult dance move though repeated practice, or playing an extended chess match in spite of fatigue and headache (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990). A second aspect of his flow theory particularly relevant to PP is the fact that his work on flow was originally published in the Journal of Humanistic Psychology (Csikszentmihalyi, 1975). In that paper, he reported that this flow experience was indistinguishable from some of the peak experiences described by Maslow. Both the place of publication and the parallel he draws to Maslow’s idea highlight the close link between his work and the tradition of the humanistic psychologists.

Another significant contribution to PP from Csikszentmihalyi has been his explicit discussion of values and their role in the good life. This explicit discussion began early in his career when collaborating in studies (Getzels & Csikszentmihalyi, 1969) of the nature of aesthetic opinion among artists and lay people. Also, much later, when he had explicitly joined the PP community, he still focused on values. In fact, he (2014) described PP as being distinguished from other areas of psychology by its attention to virtue rather than pathology.  Csikszentmihalyi never became fixated solely on happiness and always maintained a theme of virtue and social responsibility. Even when writing a business book targeting a popular audience, Csikszentmihalyi (2004) focused extensively on virtue and the responsibility business leaders have to enable good workplace experiences for their employees.

Another contribution of Csikszentmihalyi was his methodological emphasis on getting out of the typical lab while still using methods accepted within the field of psychology. He was one of the first, if not the first, researchers to conduct experience sampling research using beepers to remind participants to record their subjective experiences (Csikszentmihalyi, Larson, & Prescott, 1977); this again reflects the influence of humanistic psychology.

His research was never constrained, though, to studying only the positives of human experience. He collaborated on work with mood swings in adolescents (Larson, Csikszentmihalyi, & Graef, 1980), television addiction (Kubey & Csikszentmihalyi, 2000), time alone for adolescents (Larson & Csikszentmihalyi, 1978), and even wrote a paper (Csikszentmihalyi & Figurski, 1982) highlighting ways that self-awareness could be aversive in some contexts.

In view of the above, it is puzzling why went he along with Seligman (Seligman & Csikszentmihalyi, 2000) in explicitly distinguishing PP from its humanistic heritage. He, in fact, argued elsewhere (Csikszentmihalyi & Rathunde, 1990) that deep specialization and attention to the latest scientific findings can lead to a fragmentary understanding of the world, and that true wisdom requires a deeper holistic type of reflection. This type of reflection is exactly what characterized leaders of the humanistic movement in psychology, leaders such as Rogers and Maslow.

Right from the start of PP 1.0, Csikszentmihalyi took a pivotal role (Csikszentmihalyi & Nakamura, 2011) in defining the movement and attracting fellow scholars. He and Seligman met on a holiday and planned together ways to try to help the discipline of psychology take a positive turn. One almost comical footnote in the emergence of PP is the fact that Csikszentmihalyi joined the PP 1.0 community of scholars and even called it a new field (2014). In fact, years previously, Csikszentmihalyi (1983) had chided another scholar who developed a new science called “Eudology.” He derided that other scholar for not reading the prior research on happiness and realizing that others had already written on the topic. This same criticism could have been applied to many (though not Csikszentmihalyi) in the burgeoning field of PP 1.0.

This brief review demonstrates ways that Csikszentmihalyi took a PP perspective long before PP 1.0 was officially launched by Seligman in 1998. PP 1.0 might have been stronger if more of the adherents of this new community of scholars had learned the lessons of Csikszentmihalyi’s earlier research. His collected works are titled Flow and the Foundations of Positive Psychology: The Collected Works of Mihaly Csikszentmihalyi (2014). This title is appropriate given the way he laid groundwork for the field.

Martin E. P. Seligman

Seligman’s role within the PP movement might seem surprising given the fact that his most noted early work focused on animal learning and more specifically on the effect of electric shock on learning in dogs (Seligman & Maier, 1967). Nonetheless, even relatively early in his career, Seligman began to follow Miller’s (1969) dictate to give psychology away to non-psychologists who could apply research findings in the service of humanity. In particular, Seligman wrote a trade book on depression (Seligman, 1975) that had a hopeful tone and received much attention outside of the academy as well as within.

Seligman credits a rebuke from his five-year-old daughter with turning him away from being a grouch, away from a focus on pathology, and toward a focus on people’s strengths and on the development of those strengths (Seligman & Csikszentmihalyi, 2000). He reports that this experience shortly after being elected president of the APA changed his approach in life and his work within psychology. Regardless of the actual source of this change, Seligman became an influential leader within the PP community shortly after that event.

Seligman’s early and continuing leadership within the PP movement gives him a position of significant influence. His advocacy for the field during his 1998 presidency, his subsequent work attracting scholars to the field, his publications targeting lay audiences (e.g., Seligman, 2002), and his role in the MAPP program at the University of Pennsylvania (and in the IPPA) all put his name at the center of the community of PP scholars in North America in perception if not in practice.

Several concerns arise from Seligman’s powerful role. As with any scholar, his account of humanity is not complete, and, in particular, his early emphasis on happiness rather than eudaimonic well-being (2002) has been problematic. The early emphasis on happiness may have had the benefits of attracting many people to the PP community and appealing to a consumerist society, but it may have also triggered a backlash and contributed to the development of PP 2.0 (Wong, 2011c). An emphasis on happiness can misguide people because some research suggests that well-being is predicted more strongly by building meaning and other eudaimonic outcomes rather than by a focus on building happiness (Huta & Ryan, 2010; Steger, Kashdan, & Oishi, 2008). Admittedly, Seligman did discuss meaning in his book on authentic happiness (2002), but meaning only serves as an instrument to attain happiness. Some of Seligman’s later work, such as his book entitled Flourish (2011), introduced his PERMA model which explicitly denotes that well-being includes life meaning (the “M” in PERMA). In that same book, Seligman admitted that he used to think that happiness was the central topic of PP, but that he had changed his mind, yet even within that book, meaning received relatively little attention. The emphasis on happiness, which was admittedly much stronger in his early work in PP, can crowd out attention to other elements of well-being.

Furthermore, even after his conversion to a broader definition of well-being, Seligman does not justify why his vision of well-being deserves pre-eminence over that of other theorists or even other cultures (Wong, 2011a), and that highlights another problem with his powerful role and, more broadly, the centrality of American leadership within PP. The centrality of American scholars in his PP community may hinder the development of indigenous positive psychologies.

Seligman is a famous and influential positive psychologist in significant part because of his ability to attract big grants and big names (Wong, 2011a). At the same time, he is also a polarizing figure because of his dismissive attitude towards humanistic psychology and much other work that preceded PP. Seligman’s influence in global PP is also compromised by his imperialistic approach to spreading the “gospel” of the good life based on the American ideology of individualism.

In the final analysis, we all owe a great debit to Seligman because without his vision, leadership, and entrepreneurial success in building a community of PP scholars, we would not be contributing to this International Handbook. The past 20 years have witnessed a great deal of growing pains and expansion in PP. We believe that history will recognize his great contributions to psychology in spite of some mistakes along the way.

Christopher Peterson

In a fascinating arc, Christopher Peterson, early in his career, collaborated on research consistent with PP, then moved to more of a focus on pathology for much of his career, and then arced back and committed himself wholeheartedly to PP. In particular, one of Peterson’s earliest publications (Scott & Peterson, 1975) represented a focus much akin to PP 1.0 with its measurement of personal adjustment (rather than pathology) and measurement of a positive perspective on the world. Soon thereafter, his research focus turned toward what he would have called a “business-as-usual psychology” that focused on pathology (Peterson, 2006). Much of his work during this time focused on depression and attributional style, including his work developing the Attributional Style Questionnaire (Peterson et al., 1982; Peterson & Villanova, 1988). Ironically, this research was instrumental in moving Seligman’s research from learned helplessness to learned optimism, which was represented by an attributional style opposite to that of the depressed. Later he completed his arc back and joined Seligman in the PP movement. Peterson co-taught possibly the first university course in PP ever in 2003 at the University of Pennsylvania, and then published possibly the first full-length textbook on PP (Peterson, 2006).

Peterson deserves to be remembered for at least three major contributions to PP: his attention to virtue and character strengths (Peterson & Seligman, 2004), his attention to a broader set of positive subjective states, and his interpersonal impact. To understand the first, there is value in recalling that Seligman and Csikszentmihalyi (2000), when they introduced PP, argued for three major foci: positive subjective states (e.g., happiness), positive traits (e.g., virtues), and institutions facilitating these. Thereafter, the bulk of PP researchers and practitioners seemed focused on the first of these three: positive subjective states. In contrast, Peterson helped to keep attention on positive traits including virtues and character strengths. He collaborated on developing a modern classification system for virtues and character strengths (Peterson & Seligman, 2004). His classification system gained much traction possibly not only because of the quality of his work, but also because his collaboration with the VIA Institute in developing and distributing a strengths questionnaire allowed research with large and culturally diverse samples (e.g., McGrath, 2015; Peterson, Park, Pole, D’Andrea, & Seligman, 2008).

Peterson’s second major contribution relates to PP’s focus on positive subjective states. Many PP researchers have focused on happiness, and some on life satisfaction, but Peterson helped to broaden this with his orientations to happiness research program (e.g., Peterson, Park, & Seligman, 2005). In this work, he argued that a full life needed not only pleasure, but also meaning and engagement. This framework did not become as widely used as his VIA framework of strengths, but, nonetheless, this attention was important given his central role in the community, and may have even helped motivate Seligman’s (2011) decision to focus less on happiness and more on a broader array of positive outcomes.

Peterson’s third major contribution was interpersonal. Peterson was fascinating in part because he didn’t feel a need to pretend to always be happy even though he was a positive psychologist. As he said about himself, “I do my share of complaining, and I roll my eyes much more frequently than I smile” (Peterson, 2006, p. 19). Nevertheless, many scholars have reported being inspired and moved by Peterson’s commitment to the well-being of others (e.g., Brunwasser & Gillham, 2014; Park & Seligman, 2013; Wright, 2012). Given his prestigious location within the field, Peterson did not need to do this to succeed, but he nonetheless took time and contributed to many people’s lives. For these qualities, he was respected and loved by many psychologists, including those who are critical of PP.

Peterson was honored with a Lifetime Achievement Award from the International Network on Personal Meaning (INPM) in 2012. Unfortunately, he died prematurely about two months after the 2012 Meaning Conference. His keynote speech at the Meaning Conference was later published (Peterson & Park, 2014), which may be his last journal publication. It is worth noting that, in this paper, they emphasized the need to include meaning as an outcome measure and that a valid meaning measure should include sources of meaning and purpose (Schnell, 2009; Wong, 1998a).

Edward Diener

Ed Diener can accurately be portrayed as a courageous proponent of the need to study well-being scientifically. From very early in his career, he was warned that the scientific study of happiness was both impossible and not worthy of study, so he learned that this topic could hinder his career (Diener, 2008). As a young man, he was mystified by the apparent happiness he saw among the migrant farm workers whose lives seemed so difficult, so he wanted to study their happiness. His academic supervisor at that time would not allow that study. Diener then delayed his research on well-being until after he had earned his research credibility and tenure, when his job was more secure. Now, many years later, it is his research on subjective well-being for which he is most highly regarded, not his other research on the “approved” list of topics.

Diener has worked consistently to make his research and other work relevant to the needs of not only the privileged, but to others as well. For example, Diener collaborated on a book (Diener, Lucas, Schimmak, & Helliwell, 2009) describing ideas for applying research findings to develop social policy that could improve well-being for millions of people. In related work, he also argued that governments should consistently survey the psychological well-being of their populace to measure the impact of their policies and also to identify subgroups with the greatest needs (Diener & Diener, 2011). This attention to the needs of the populace rather than just to the needs of the elite who can afford therapy or who will read his writings, also deserves praise.

Diener has also drawn attention to cultural differences in causes of well-being and even in the nature of well-being (e.g., Diener et al., in press), and he has also at least occasionally given credit to humanistic psychologists for the relevance of their ideas to modern explorations of well-being (Diener, 1984; Diener, 2008; Tay & Diener, 2011), though we could argue that the humanist psychologists deserve even more credit.

One concern with Diener’s work has been his consistent emphasis on subjective well-being as the ultimate indicator of well-being. His definition of subjective well-being heavily weights frequent positive affect, infrequent negative affect, and life satisfaction (Diener, 1984; Diener, 2000). Though these are important indicators of well-being, this approach over-represents hedonic indicators of well-being and under-represents eudaimonic indicators of well-being (Proctor, Tweed, & Morris, 2015). In contrast, the eudaimonic orientations that Diener has under-attended to seem particularly important because they may produce longer lasting well-being than do hedonic orientations (Huta & Ryan, 2010; Steger, Kashdan, & Oishi, 2008). More recently, Diener (Su, Tay, & Diener, 2014), like Seligman (2011), has broadened his focus to a wider set of indicators of well-being.

In spite of this concern, Diener’s research is highly valued and has relevance beyond the academy. These, and his courageous perseverance in the face of potential career harm, deserve to be remembered.

Sonja Lyubomirsky

As with many of the notable positive psychologists described here, Lyubomirsky began her career with a focus on psychopathology, and, in her case, with a focus on the relation between rumination (Lyubomirsky & Nolen-Hoeksema, 1993) and strategies for reducing dysphoria (Lyubomirsky & Nolen-Hoeksema, 1995). Then, she reoriented and wrote a paper describing a measure of happiness (Lyubomirsky & Lepper, 1999). Then, for the rest of her career, she has focused mainly on exploring the sources and causes of happiness.

Lyubomirsky’s early work within the stream of PP was distinguished by its focus on hedonic happiness, largely to the exclusion of other indicators of well-being (e.g., Lyubomirsky, 2001). Issues related to eudaimonia received little attention in most of her early work, and as a result, in spite of the value of her work, she matched some of the imbalance within the rest of PP 1.0.

One of the most significant contributions from Lyubomirsky has been her work popularizing the nature of PP interventions. She has written popular books on strategies for increasing happiness (Lyubomirsky, 2008, 2013). In this way, she has become a significant face of PP for many people who lack training in the topic. In some ways, though, in spite of the benefits she has provided to the populace, her prominent role may have exacerbated the tendency for people to attend to mainly the hedonic-oriented values of PP rather than the work on eudaimonia.

Another very significant contribution has been Lyubomirsky’s early (Lyubomirsky & Lepper, 1999) and ongoing (Lyubomirsky, Dickerhoof, Boehm, & Sheldon, 2011) awareness that people may differ in terms of activities and achievements that produce hedonic well-being. In recent years, she has had an unusual sensitivity to the fact that PP interventions may be ineffective or harmful if implemented with the wrong population at the wrong time. She has presented some evidence that motivation and effort to become happy will moderate the extent to which individual happiness inductions will be successful (Lyubomirsky et al., 2011) and also that other factors, including some that may change over time, will influence the quality of fit between interventions and potential users (Lyubomirsky & Layous, 2013).

Barbara L. Fredrickson

Barbara Fredrickson’s involvement in the formal PP community began early. She attended Seligman’s first PP conference in Akumal, Mexico for which he invited only eighteen elite young scholars (Seligman, 1999b). Also, she was one of the authors of the Positive Psychology Manifesto that was produced out of the Akumal conferences (Sheldon, Fredrickson, Rathunde, & Csikszentmihalyi, 2000).

Fredrickson is most noted for her broaden and build theory (Fredrickson, 1998). That theory suggests that positive emotions help overcome tendencies toward a narrow focus of attention and thereby help people see a broader range of possible thoughts and actions available to them. The theory also suggests that this broadening then helps people build resources and move toward emotional well-being (Fredrickson, 1998). Since the publication of that theory, she has gathered some further support for the broadening effect of positive emotions (e.g., Fredrickson & Branigan, 2005) and for the impact of positivity on well-being (e.g., Kok et al., 2013; Kok & Fredrickson, 2015). She has been successful in spreading this message beyond the community of PP scholars to a wider scientific audience (e.g., Fredrickson, 2003).

Fredrickson’s work has not been without controversy. Some work on the relation between positive emotion and health has been criticized (Heathers, Brown, Coyne, & Friedman, 2015) though she and her collaborator have sought to alleviate these concerns (Kok & Fredrickson, 2015). Her claim that a critical ratio of at least 2.9 positive emotions to negative emotion demarks a flourishing individual (Fredrickson & Losada, 2005) has been criticized (Brown, Sokal & Friedman, 2013). Finally, her research on well-being and genomics (Fredrickson et al., 2013) has also been questioned (Brown, MacDonald, Samanta, Friedman, & Coyne, 2014). In spite of these controversies, she remains one of the most creative and influential positive psychologists.

In more recent work, like many of the other positive psychologists mentioned here, Fredrickson seems to have reduced her focus on hedonia and taken more interest in work related to eudaimonia. That includes work on meditation (van Cappellen, May, Isgett, & Fredrickson, 2016), mindfulness, and even meaning. She and her collaborators (Garland, Farb, Goldin, & Fredrickson, 2015) argued that the cognitive flexibility induced by mindfulness enhances the meaning-making process. In this sense, her trajectory within PP is somewhat like that of Seligman and Diener, with an early focus on hedonia and a later transition toward attention on a broader set of indicators of well-being.

Richard M. Ryan

Richard Ryan is Professor at the Institute for Positive Psychology and Education at the Australian Catholic University and Research Professor in Psychology at the University of Rochester in New York. He is co-developer (with Edward Deci) of the Self-Determination Theory (SDT), a theory of human motivation that has generated a great deal of research and has been applied to many areas, including education, clinical practice, sports psychology, and eudaimonia (Ryan, Curren, & Deci, 2013; Taubes, 2010).

Ryan and Deci both rank among the top one percent of researchers (Thomson Reuters, 2015). Ryan is recognized as one of the eminent psychologists of the modern era (Diener, Oishi, & Park, 2014). In 2012, Ryan was honored with a Lifetime Achievement Award from the INPM (International Network on Personal Meaning, 2012).

Self-determination is based on intrinsic motivation, such as feelings of autonomy and competence. Patterson and Joseph (2007) were correct in seeing a connection between SDT and Rogers’ person-centered theory because they provide similar perspectives and meta-theoretical assumptions. There is a great deal of cross-cultural data supporting the universality of the human needs for autonomy, agency, and freedom (Chirkov, Ryan, & Sheldon, 2011). When these basic needs are frustrated or thwarted, people will become more vulnerable to mental illnesses (Vansteenkiste & Ryan, 2013).

SDT was around in the 1980s, well before Seligman launched his brand of PP in 1998. Ryan and Deci were not included in the group that Seligman called the “Senior Scholars” in Seligman’s original network of PP (Seligman, 1999b). Yet, SDT remains the only general theory of human motivation that has received considerable empirical support and provided a very helpful conceptual framework to enhance eudemonic happiness, well-being, and personal growth and, at the same time, reduce vulnerability. The strength of SDT lies in its recognition of human nature and people’s basic needs for freedom, autonomy, and relationships.

Carol D. Ryff

Carol Ryff is Director of the Institute on Aging and Hilldale Professor of Psychology at the University of Wisconsin-Madison. Ryff has never been part of the PP community and has been critical of PP (Ryff, 2003). Yet, her research on psychological well-being, and positive health has been widely cited because of its broad scholarship, solid empirical foundation, and cross-cultural contribution. Although she is mostly known from her theory and research on well-being, she has also been impactful in intervention strategies and educational programs (Ryff, 2014a, b; Ryff et al., 2014; Ruini & Ryff, 2016).

Multidimensional Assessment of Psychological Well-Being. Ryff’s (1989, 2014a) research on the six domains of psychological well-being was both ground-breaking and seminal. Her model was based on integrating ideas from existential, humanistic, developmental, and clinical psychology. She benefited from their deep insights of the bright side of human nature, as shown in Figure 1.

Figure 1. The six main components of psychological well-being (Ryff, 2014a).

Figure 1. The six main components of psychological well-being (Ryff, 2014a).

Different from most PP assessment tools, which are based on short and simple self-reports of undefined psychological constructs (for a critique, see Wong & Roy, in press), Ryff’s well-being scale is multidimensional and lengthy; the long form of the scale consists of 84 questions and the medium form 54 questions (Seifert, 2005). This scale is much longer than the typical PP measure, yet it has been translated to more than 30 different languages and generated more than 500 publications because of its theoretical rootedness and universal appeal.

Consistent with Aristotle (1908) and existential positive human functioning (Frankl, 1985; Wong, 2009a), Ryff emphasized “know thyself and become what you are” as the key to her eudaimonic approach to psychological well-being (Ryff & Singer, 2008). Ryff also provided biological correlates (cardiovascular, neuroendocrine, immune) of a eudaimonic well-being that is rich in meaning and purpose, quality relationships, and continued growth (Ryff, 2012; Ryff & Singer, 1998).

Holistic Multidisciplinary Approach to Human Resilience. Another theme in Ryff’s research has been human resilience—the human capacity to maintain or regain well-being in the face of cumulative adversity and underlying neurobiology. Her holistic multidimensional research integrates several levels of analysis: sociodemographic characteristics, psychosocial resources, life stresses, health behaviors and practices, neurobiological risk and protective factors, and health outcomes with extensive psychosocial and biomarker assessments (Ryff, 2014b).

The main findings from longitudinal studies of aging have shown that those who remain purposefully engaged with life experience numerous health benefits. Such findings demonstrate the importance of promoting eudaimonic well-being and resilience across a broad segment of the population, including the socioeconomically disadvantaged and those suffering from cancer and child abuse (Ryff, 2014a, 2016; Ruini & Ryff, 2016).

Cross-Cultural Sensitivity. Since 1995, Ryff and her team have researched factors that influence health and well-being from middle age through old age via a study called MIDUS (Mid-Life in the U.S. National Study of Americans). She has also been involved in a parallel study in Japan. She has directly compared well-being, resilience, and adult development in Japan and the U.S. (Ryff et al., 2015). Thus, her third major contribution to PP is that she is keenly aware that well-being is rooted in culture, and hence interventions need to be sensitive to cultural contexts (Ryff et al., 2014). For example, she and her associates (Curhan et al., 2014) discovered that Japanese people were less affected by negative emotions than their American counterparts. This difference can be easily understood in terms of yin and yang differences in coping. Eastern cultures are more attuned to the tragedies and hardships of life and have learned to cope with patience and endurance—the yin aspects of adaptation and are less likely to expect to immediately eradicate every life problem (Lin, 1935; Tweed, White, & Lehman, 2002).

Ryff stands out as a model researcher on well-being and has avoided the many mistakes of American PP (Wong & Roy, in press). The above three areas of her contributions to PP happen to be among the defining characteristics of existential PP (Wong, 2009a) and PP 2.0 (Wong, 2011c). That is why she was introduced as the “Mother of PP 2.0” at the 2016 International Meaning Conference when she was honored with a Lifetime Achievement Award.

Robert A. Emmons

Robert Emmons is a Professor of Psychology at the University of California, Davis and the founding editor-in-chief of The Journal of Positive Psychology. He is the world’s leading scientific expert on gratitude (Emmons, 2007, 2013). However, to him, gratitude is more than just a valid instrument to enhance happiness, because “gratitude heals, energizes, and transforms lives” (Emmons, 2016) and the nature of gratitude is spiritual (Emmons, 2010; Emmons & Hill, 2001). The PP literature often ignores Emmons’s major contributions to spirituality and meaning. In this chapter, we want to highlight these larger contributions that pave the way for PP 2.0.

Spiritual Beliefs and Strivings Hold the Key to Well-Being. Like Viktor Frankl (1985), Emmons recognizes that spirituality is a core dimension of being human. Thus, people are viewed as bio-psycho-social-spiritual beings rather than mere animals. This high and noble view of who we are should fundamentally influence PP research regarding how we should live our lives. This deeper understanding of human nature elevates the academic discourse from the simple empirical question of what exercises may enhance well-being and happiness to the fundamental theoretical question of how to be becoming fully functioning human beings with spiritual strivings.

Consistent with humanistic-existential thinking, Emmons (2005) emphasizes the vital role of religion and spirituality in human existence by “establishing goals and value systems that potentially pertain to all aspects of a person’s life” (p. 731). By framing religion and spirituality in terms of goal-directed behavior, he makes a compelling case that meaning is not a matter of subjective feeling or thinking, but a matter of goal-directed actions towards ultimate concerns (Emmons, 1986, 1999).

Ultimate concerns are “concerns over ultimate questions of meaning and existence, purpose and values” (Emmons, 2005, p. 737), which express themselves in personal goals. These spiritual goals are characterized by striving to transcend the self and serving something greater than the self, by seeking a relationship with a higher power, and by integrating the individual with “larger and more complex units” (Emmons, 2005, p. 736); these characteristic are highly similar to Wong’s conceptualization of self-transcendence (Wong, 2016b).

Emmons’ meaning-centered approach to personality and well-being is evident in his definition of self-identity in terms of the “pursuit of personally significant goals in general, and goals of a religious and spiritual nature in particular” (Emmons, 2005, p. 732). His research has demonstrated that people with spiritual goals “tend to experience their lives as worthwhile, unified and meaningful” (Emmons, 1999, p. 104).

Finally, consistent with PP 2.0 (Wong, 2011c), Emmons has recognized that implicit worldviews and values systems—which give rise to spiritual strivings—are essential not only to human flourishing, but also to resilience in coping with adversity and traumas. For instance, Emmons’ (2005) research with persons with neuromuscular disease showed that when their strivings were centered on the sacred, “they [were] likely to experience life as fulfilling, meaningful, and purposeful, even in the face of a deteriorating and disabling physical condition” (p. 742).

Gratitude is an Expression of Spirituality. Gratitude is a natural expression of a spiritual life—a life that is fully aware of God or a Higher Power as the source of life and all its blessings. When gratitude is employed as an instrument to enhance personal happiness, such as paying a gratitude visit or writing a gratitude letter, it does not always work because it depends on each individual’s personal and cultural context (Parks & Biswas-Diener, 2013). Furthermore, a life focused merely on pursuing happiness whether through gratitude or other means, can undermine the very happiness that is being sought (Martin, 2008). However, when gratitude is cultivated as a spiritual practice, it works in every culture, because it has been an ancient spiritual discipline practiced in every religion. The simplest form of such practice is to count our blessings on a daily basis, not just for being alive, but also for the many benefits we have received (Wong, 2016c).

Emmons (2010) was especially correct in pointing out that the nature of gratitude has two components:

(1) It’s an affirmation of goodness. We affirm that there are good things in the world, gifts and benefits we’ve received. … (2) We recognize the sources of this goodness as being outside of ourselves. … I think true gratitude involves a humble dependence on others: We acknowledge that other people—or even higher powers, if you’re of a spiritual mindset—gave us many gifts, big and small, to help us achieve the goodness in our lives. (para. 9-10).

Contributions to PP 2.0. Emmons (2005) is fully aware that “what contributes to the self-perceived well-being of an individual might be detrimental to the well-being of others” (p. 742). Thus, a truly meaningful and fulfilling life is not just about the individual, but also about others and future generations. Emmons is also cognizant that spiritual striving towards the sacred may entail guilt and shame, but “dissatisfaction can be desirable if it is used as fuel for constructive life change. Even usually positive characteristics can have harmful consequences” (p. 742).

Finally, in agreement with Viktor Frankl (1985), Emmons (2005) believes that direct pursuit of happiness can be counterproductive, because “research indicates that happiness is often a by-product of participating in worthwhile projects and activities that do not have as their primary focus the attainment of happiness” (p. 733).

When we consider the totality of Emmons’ research on spirituality and meaning, we would agree that gratitude is not just an instrument to enhance happiness, but an overflow of a heart full of thanksgiving for the blessings of serving something greater than oneself. A great deal is owed to Emmons’ research and insights for the development of PP 2.0 (Ivtzan, Lomas, Hefferon, & Worth, 2015; Wong, 2011c). Like Carol Ryff, he is an important researcher responsible for the transition from PP “as usual” to PP 2.0.

Notable Positive Psychologists in Canada

Canada is not short of positive psychologists influential in research and/or teaching. Tayyab Rashid (www.tayyabrashid.com) is well known for his research on positive psychotherapy (Rashid & Howes, 2016). Robert J. Vallerand (www.lrcs.uqam.ca/rjvaller_en.htm), Professor and the Director of the Research Laboratory on Social Behavior at the Université du Québec à Montréal, is known for his research on the psychology of passion (Vallerand, 2015). Chris Davis of Carlton University is known for his research on meaning-making in tragic situations (Davis, Nolen-Hoeksema, & Larson, 1998). Another major player in meaning research is Gary Reker (Reker, Peacock, & Wong, 1987). Finally, Kenneth Hart is known for being the first Canadian professor to develop a university course on PP (Hart & Sasso, 2011).

However, we decided to focus on Paul Wong for his influence on the PP 2.0 movement and Veronika Huta for her systematic study on eudaimonic happiness, because of their potential impact on the future of PP in Canada.

Paul T. P. Wong

Suffering is both the source and inspiration of PP for Paul Wong, because of his experience in poverty and adversities (Wong, 2016d). In his long and varied research over four decades, the leitmotif has been the same: How to overcome the dark side of life in order to be a fully functioning human being. Thus, PP 2.0 has always been his approach to PP. Although he is mainly known for his research on meaning, he has made significant contributions to research, theory, assessment, and interventions across several domains.

Effective Coping with Frustration and Pain. In his first 10 years of research, Wong’s main interest was in persistence. Through a variety of intermittent reinforcement and punishment schedules, he was able to teach animals persistence and flexibility in overcoming prolonged frustration and pain (Wong, 1977, 1995). This line of research provided an animal model of the PP of optimism and persistence (Wong, 1979, 2006a). Wong’s research on persistence has also led to Rosenbaum’s (1990) development of learned resourcefulness in human beings. These research findings provide the empirical basis for his deep-and-wide hypothesis of negative emotions (Wong, 2012a, in press-a).

Effective Coping with Stress, Aging, and Dying. Wong’s main contribution to stress and coping research is the resource-congruence model (Wong, 1993; Wong, Reker, & Peacock, 2006) and the Stress Appraisal Measure (Peacock & Wong, 1990). Wong also made the case that effective coping needs to be a part of PP 2.0 (Wong, Wong, & Scott, 2006).

In the area of positive aging and dying, Wong’s main contribution was to reorient successful aging from biological factors to psychological and spiritual factors (Reker & Wong, 1988, 2012; Wong, 1989; Wong & Watt, 1991). Wong and associates developed the Death Attitude Profile-Revised, which includes three kinds of death acceptance as positive ways to face death (Wong, Reker, & Gesser, 1994).

Wong’s demonstration of spontaneous attribution, both causal and existential (Wong, 1991; Wong & Weiner, 1981), contributes to the understanding of meaning making. As well, Wong discovered that internal and external control are two separate dimensions rather than two opposite poles on the same continuum (Wong & Sproule, 1984). Thus, positive and negative emotions and experiences can also be conceptualized as two separate interactive dimensions, which led to the dialectical framework of PP2.0 (Wong, 2009b, 2011c, 2016a).

Contribution to Assessments. In addition to those mentioned above, Wong has developed the following instruments, which can be used for both research and therapy: Personal Meaning Profile (Wong, 1998a) and Personal Meaning Profile-Brief (McDonald, Wong, & Gingras, 2012); Trent Attribution Profile (Wong & Sproule, 1984); Coping Schemas Inventory (Wong, Reker, & Peacock, 2006); Meaning Mindset Measure (Wong, 2012b); Self-Transcendence Measure (Wong, 2016e); Life Orientation Scale (Wong, 2014); and Life Attitude Scale (Wong, 2009c).

Contribution to International Psychology. His contribution to international psychology is threefold: (1) his emphasis on the importance of cross-cultural perspectives in stress and coping and PP (Leong & Wong, 2003; Wong, 2013b; Wong & Ujimoto, 1998; Wong & Wong, 2006); (2) his organization of the International Biennial Meaning Conferences since 2000 (www.meaning.ca/conference), which have been attended by psychologists from over 30 countries; and (3) his research on death acceptance and the meaning of life, which has influenced Taiwan’s Life and Death Education (S. Chang, 2016) and the development of a meaning-oriented positive education (S. Chang, 2016; Wong, 2013c), and which was honored with a Lifetime Achievement Award in Life Education by the National Taipei University of Nursing and Health.

Contribution to Existential Competencies and Meaning Therapy. Aware of the prevalence of existential concerns (Frankl, 1985; Yalom, 1980), Wong (2016f) advocated the need to develop existential competencies in all mental health workers, regardless of their therapeutic modalities.

Wong’s (2010, 2015) meaning therapy represents a comprehensive approach to transforming existential anxieties to positive motivation and strategies for healing and flourishing. The main existential competences include the dual-systems model (Wong, 2012a), meaning-mindset (Wong, 2012b), and self-transcendence (Wong, 2016b). Meaning therapy is also known as existential positive interventions (Wong, 2016c). Wong’s book, entitled Human Quest for Meaning (Wong, 2012c), also succeeded in bringing together an impressive list of international scholars who contributed a number of accessible chapters explicating of the role of meaning in research and therapy.

In sum, Wong is a positive psychologist shaped by humanistic and existential concerns, because of his own suffering.

Veronika Huta

Veronika Huta earned her Ph.D. in clinical psychology from McGill University, but chose to pursue research in eudaimonia. The main reason for choosing Huta over other more established positive psychologists in Canada in this chapter is to include a rising star whose research program shows promise of becoming an important part of PP 2.0 (Huta, 2016a).

Her eudaimonia research focuses on the existential-spiritual aspect of the human condition and emphasizes the active cultivation of virtue, not just the identification of signature character strengths. She is also exceptionally strong when it comes to theoretical integration, and has been working to contribute to the theoretical foundation for the eudaimonic-hedonic distinction (Huta, 2016b).

Huta (2015, 2016b, c) is mostly known for her comprehensive and systematic research on eudaimonic versus hedonic orientations, which represent differences in meanings, values, priorities, motives, ideals, and goals that guide the person’s chosen behaviors. In her definition of eudaimonia, she does not shy away from including virtue and meaning as explicit components. Based on a review of the literature (Huta & Waterman, 2014), she has distilled the definition to a combination of four elements: (1) pursuing virtue and excellence, (2) meaning and big picture thinking, (3) authenticity and self-honesty, and (4) growth and maturation.

When studying well-being outcomes, from the beginning she has included a more comprehensive package than subjective well-being, one which includes the higher, more complex, more existential aspects of experience: meaning (feelings of significance, value, broad implications, personal resonance, and purpose), elevating experience (awe, inspiration, moral elevation, and transcendence or sense of connection with a greater whole), and self-connectedness, all of which have proven to relate more strongly to eudaimonic than hedonic pursuits (Huta, 2016d; Huta & Ryan, 2010). Her research on the eudaimonic orientation is another demonstration of the advantages of the person-centered holistic approach to PP.

She has also developed the most comprehensive measure of eudaimonic and hedonic orientations, called the HEMA (Hedonic and Eudaimonic Motives for Activities) scale. The original (Huta & Ryan, 2010) had nine items, and it was later updated to a 10-item version to include all four elements of her definition (Huta, 2016b). Research with the HEMA scale has shown that eudaimonic pursuits are associated with more positive contributions to others, society, and the environment, and more abstract thinking and future time perspective, thus consistent with the lifestyle characterized by self-transcendence (Huta, 2016b, e; Wong, 2016b).

Wong and Roy (in press) have argued that ultimately a componential approach of emphasizing thinking, feeling, and behavior will not advance PP without a comprehensive theory about human nature and the individual as a holistic unity. Therefore, they have advocated PP 2.0 by emphasizing the humanistic values or noble aspects of human nature, such as the meaning mindset as a spiritually-oriented worldview (Frankl, 1985; Wong, 2011c) and the primary motivation of pursuing self-transcendence as the end value. We have always maintained that eudaimonic well-being results from such worldview and value (Wong, 2014, 2016b).

Huta’s latest research with her student Braaten on worldviews (Braaten & Huta, 2016) is an even clearer indication that she has extended eudaimonic research to a metaphysical level of spirituality and meaning. Their research supports our contention that worldviews matter. Human beings are not just creatures motivated by the pursuit of pleasure and avoidance of pain, as proposed by those embracing a naturalist hedonic worldview, but spiritual beings struggling for meaning and purpose and exploring the moral and ethical implications of their actions and goals.

Braaten and Huta have developed (and continue refining) the most comprehensive measure of worldviews to date, based on Koltko-Rivera’s (2004) extensive literature review. They have empirically demonstrated the importance of worldviews for meaning and well-being. They have shown that people are more likely to pursue eudaimonia—serving a greater purpose, achieving excellence in virtue and competence, fulfilling one’s unique potentials, and being true to oneself—when they embrace the existential belief that the universe and people exist for a greater purpose. Thus, two different lines of research—the search for meaning (Wong, 2012c, 2014) and the pursuit of eudaimonia (Huta, 2016a, b)—conducted independently have arrived at basically the same conclusion, adding credence to the meaning hypothesis of human flourishing.

Future of Positive Psychology in North America

PP will stay for the long haul as a sub-discipline of psychology, just like abnormal psychology (Wong, 2011c). From the perspective of PP 2.0, the future of PP represents the development of the new science of investigating how to bring out the best in people and society in spite of the dark side of human existence through dialectical principles. Here are some specific trends for the future.

Future Trends of PP in North America

From Hedonic to Eudaimonic Happiness. The happiness craze associated with the smiley face of PP 1.0 is over (Ivtzan, Lomas, Hefferon, & Worth, 2015). The authors of this chapter have thus far argued that the good life involves more than just hedonic happiness. Self-help books on how to be happy (e.g., Fredrickson, 2009; Lyubomirsky, 2008; Rubin, 2009) may be perceived by some as books for privileged white people (e.g., Coyne, 2013; Ehrenreich, 2010). We foresee more research on eudaimonic happiness along the lines of research by Ryan (self-determination), Ryff (psychological well-being), Huta (eudaimonic well-being), and Wong (meaning and self-transcendence).

From Mindfulness to Self-Transcendence. We also do not believe that mindfulness will be the new panacea for PP, as many have suggested (Harris, 2008; Weiss & Hickman, n.d.; Williams & Penman, 2012). Although gaining peace of mind through the secular approach to the Buddhist practice of mindful meditation is helpful, it has limited benefits because it does not involve any change in core beliefs and way of life (Wong, 2006b). We hasten to add that Buddhism’s emphasis on no-self is only one of the several pathways to attain inner peace and self-transcendence (Wong, 2016a, b).

For example, Wong (2012b) emphasizes the meaning-mindset, which will enable us to see life as potentially meaningful. He also stresses the motivation to pursue self-transcendence, which will reorient us from self-interest to caring for others in a responsible way (Wong, 2016b). Available research has shown that self-transcendence is an important factor for meaning in life (Wong, 1998b; Smith, 2017), tragic optimism (Wong, 2009c; Wong & McDonald, 2002), and flow (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990).

From American-Centered Approach to Indigenous Emphasis. Numerous psychologists have argued that PP is rooted in specific cultural contexts (E. Chang, Downey, Hirsch, & Lin, 2016; Kim, Yang, & Hwang, 2006; Leong & Wong, 2003; Wong, 2013b). Each ethnic/cultural group may have their own moral visions, which “inform people about what is worthy, good, and desirable, and about what constitutes the good, virtue, morality, health, and well-being” (Christopher, Wendt, Marecek, & Goodman, 2014, p. 5).

We foresee the development of indigenous PP for Chinese people (Wong, 2016a), aboriginal communities (McCormick & Wong, 2006), African Americans (Caldwell-Colbert, Parks, & Eshun, 2009), and Latina/o students (Vela, Lu, Penz, & Hinojosa, 2015). We also encourage research on well-being and life satisfaction that is sensitive to cultural factors (Diener & Suh, 2000; Ujimoto, Nishio, Wong, & Lam, 1993; Wong & Ujimoto, 1998). Such research has a twofold objective: (a) discovering what is unique about each indigenous culture and (b) discovering what is universal or what is human nature.

From Lab Research to Real Life Experiences. The main problem with lab research is that it may not have external validity and may not be relevant to real life situations. The second problem is that all kinds of painful human conditions cannot be studied in the lab due to ethical concerns. Therefore, for PP 2.0 research, it is essential to study real life situations. Csikszentmihalyi’s technique of sampling subjective experiences in real life is a promising approach. Biographical and historical research provides yet another fertile ground to study topics of PP 2.0.

Another argument in favor of this change in methodology is the need to honor the subjective experiences of research participants. Rating scores of well-being and meaning in life are difficult to interpret unless we know the mental state or life circumstances of participants while completing the rating scores (Wong, in press-b). Therefore, future research based on self-reports needs to have at least a sub-sample that reports what is going through their mind while completing the self-ratings.

From Single-Minded Focus on Positive Variables to Dialectic Interactions. PP’s contribution to well-being will be limited if we continue to demonstrate empirically what we all know experientially, such as how being close to nature, listening to music, engaging in interesting activities, or being together with family and friends makes us happy. Such a pedestrian approach to documenting an endless list of activities that can make us happy without knowing the underlying conditions or cultural differences will not advance the science of PP.

It is a more challenging scientific task to investigate how positive variables interact with negative life circumstances to increase our well-being, character strengths, or group harmony, simply because there is very little research on dual-system interactions (Wong, 2012a) and we also have less personal experience in regards to dialectical outcomes.

Major Obstacles that May Prevent Positive Psychology from Growing

We can readily identify the following obstacles to future growth.

Commercialization of Positive Psychology. In the long run, the commercialization of PP beyond scientific support will weaken its credibility. We can understand why such a market is necessary because increasing numbers of positive coaches need to make a living, but, eventually, it could bring discredit to the entire field of PP (Wong & Roy, in press).

Non-Collaboration Between Different Groups. The lack of willingness to collaborate between the PP and humanistic communities has hindered both groups from realizing their full potentials. One way to overcome this divide is to do quantitative research on humanistic constructs (e.g., Proctor, Tweed, & Morris, 2016) or to make full use of the mixed methodology (Wong, in press-b). Another approach is to forge a closer tie between these two traditions by bringing them together in collaborative projects, such as the INPM’s International Biennial Meaning Conferences (www.meaning.ca/conference). Finally, a humble approach to science and a noble purpose for the greater good will facilitate collaboration between diverse groups.

Disparity Between Haves and Have-Nots. Overall, PP has catered to those who have the time and money to pursue happiness projects or benefit from positive coaches. It is hoped that PP 2.0 will develop interventions to help the homeless (Tweed, Biswas-Diener, & Lehman, 2012) and the hopeless (Wong, 2009c; Wong & McDonald, 2002), and equip people with better coping skills to grow and to become better persons through adversities (Wong, Wong, & Scott, 2006). Furthermore, PP has a responsibility to address the issue of poverty (Biswas-Diener & Patterson, 2011) and social injustice (Eidelson, Lyubansky, Malley-Morrison, 2014).

Changes Needed to Increase Impact of PP

From the perspective of PP 2.0, the following things need to be achieved:

  1. We need to get across the message that PP is not just for people in neutral or positive territories, but also for the suffering masses (Wong, 2007, 2011c). It is a branch of psychology designed to bring out the best angels in our nature as a way of overcoming the dark side of life and human nature. Simply focusing on character strengths by itself is not enough, if we do not confront the evils of oppression, exploitation, and violence.
  2. We need to give PP away to the community as Wong has done for many years (Wong, 2012d). We need to adopt a grassroots approach to educate people on the principles of meaningful living (Wong, 2016g) as a way to prevent mental illness and promote human flourishing. When more people have acquired the basic principles of meaning and practiced them, they will be able to live more positive lives and create a better society.
  3. We need to develop an alternative approach to positive education—positive education 2.0—as advocated by Wong (2013d) and S. Chang (2016). In a way that is different from the first wave of positive education, which emphasizes the science of happiness and signature strengths (Seligman, Ernst, Gillham, Reivich, & Linkins, 2009), positive education 2.0 seeks to cultivate a sense of personal and social responsibility (Linley & Maltby, 2009); affirm the inherent value, meaning, and dignity of individual life (Frankl, 1985); and emphasize the practical wisdom of living well (Schwartz & Sharpe, 2010) in light of the brevity of life. In Taiwan, this kind of positive education is called “life education”; it has become a national policy for all levels of public schools to include life education as part of their academic curriculum (S. Chang, 2016; Huang, Wang, & Lin, 2011). Thus, positive education 2.0 not only teaches students important life skills and moral values, but also imparts in students a sense of civic virtue and social responsibility necessary for a robust democracy.
  4. We need to secure funding for research based on a humble and holistic approach to science (Templeton, 1998) to carry out systematic research within the framework of PP 2.0.

The challenges of PP to make life better for all includes the following (Wong, 2016h):

  1. To better the lives of all people and nurture their potentials, regardless of their circumstances and cultural backgrounds.
  2. To repair the worst and bring out the best, with a focus on the human potential for growth.
  3. To integrate negatives and positives to optimize well-being.
  4. To study how global beliefs and values affect people’s eudaimonic well-being and human functioning.
  5. To minimize or transform the downside of the bright side, and optimize or realize the upside of the dark side.
  6. To cultivate the meaning-mindset and the humanistic values of kindness and unconditional acceptance of all people.
  7. To study how death awareness can contribute to personal transformation.
  8. To develop better objective measures of both short-term and long-term well-being for individuals and society.
  9. To enhance well-being throughout the lifespan, including the end-of-life stage.
  10. To identify and research a host of variables related to both yin and yang.
  11. To contain and transform the pervasive evil in society to serve the common good.
  12. To cultivate inner goodness and develop valid measures of goodness as an outcome.

References

  1. Aristotle. (1908). The Nicomachean ethics. (W. D. Ross, Trans.). Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press.
  2. Biswas-Diener, R. & Patterson. L. (2011). Positive psychology and poverty. In R. Biswas-Diener (Ed.), Positive psychology as social change (pp. 125-140). New York, NY: Springer.
  3. Braaten, A., & Huta, V. (2016). How fundamental worldviews about life and the world relate to eudaimonia, hedonia, and the experience of meaning. Paper presented at the 9th Biennial International Meaning Conference, Toronto, ON.
  4. Brown, N. J. L., MacDonald, D. A., Samanta, M. P., Friedman, H. L., & Coyne, J. C. (2014). A critical reanalysis of the relationship between genomics and well-being. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111, 12705–12709. doi:10.1073/pnas.1407057111
  5. Brown, J. L., Sokal, A. D., & Friedman, H. L. (2013). The complex dynamics of wishful thinking: The critical positivity ratio. American Psychologist, 68(9), 801-813. doi:10.1037/a0032850
  6. Brunwasser, S., & Gillham, J. (2014). In memory of Chris Peterson: Introduction to special issue. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 10(1), 1-2.
  7. Caldwell-Colbert, A. T., Parks, F. M., & Eshun, S. (2009) Positive psychology: African American strengths, resilience, and protective factors. In H. A. Neville, B. M. Tynes, & S. O. Utsey (Eds.), Handbook of African American psychology (pp. 375-384). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
  8. Chang, E. C., Downey, C. A., Hirsch, J. K., & Lin, N. J. (Eds.). (2016). Positive psychology in racial and ethnic groups: Theory, research, and practice. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
  9. Chang, S-M. (2016). The development and implementation of life education in Taiwan: A meaning-centered positive education. Meaning Conference. Retrieved from http://meaning.ca/conference/shu-mei-chang/
  10. Chirkov, V. I., Ryan, R. M., & Sheldon, K. M. (Eds.). (2011). Human autonomy in cross-cultural context: Perspectives on the psychology of agency, freedom, and well-being. New York, NY: Springer.
  11. Christopher, J. C., Wendt, D. C., Marecek, J., & Goodman, D. M. (2014). Critical cultural awareness: Contributions to a globalizing psychology. American Psychologist, 69(7), 645-655.
  12. Coyne, J. (2013, August 21). Positive psychology is mainly for rich white people. PLOS Blogs. Retrieved from http://blogs.plos.org/mindthebrain/2013/08/21/positive-psychology-is-mainly-for-rich-white-people/
  13. Coyne, J. C. (2014a, Oct. 28). Positive psychology interventions for depressive symptoms. PLOS Blogs. Retrieved from http://blogs.plos.org/mindthebrain/2014/10/28/positive-psychology-interventions-depressive-symptoms/
  14. Coyne, J. C. (2014b, Nov. 18). Failing grade for highly cited meta-analysis of positive psychology interventions. PLOS Blogs. Retrieved from http://blogs.plos.org/mindthebrain/2014/11/18/failing-grade-highly-cited-meta-analysis-positive-psychology-interventions/
  15. Coyne, J. C., Tennen, H., & Ranchor, A. V. (2010). Positive psychology in cancer care: A story line resistant to evidence. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 39(1), 35-42.
  16. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1970). Sociological implications in the thought of Teilhard de Chardin. Zygon Journal of Religion & Science, 5(2), 130-146.
  17. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1975). Play and intrinsic rewards. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 15(3), 41-63.
  18. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1983). A “Columbus fallacy.” New Ideas in Psychology, 1(3), 313-314.
  19. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New York, NY: Harper & Row.
  20. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Flow. In A. E. Kazdin (Ed.), Encyclopedia of psychology (pp. 381-382). Washington DC: American Psychological Association.
  21. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2004). Good business: Leadership, flow, and the making of meaning. New York, NY: Viking.
  22. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2014). Flow and the foundations of positive psychology: The collected works of Mihaly Csikszentmihalyi. New York, NY: Springer.
  23. Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Figurski, T. J. (1982). Self-awareness and aversive experience in everyday life. Journal of Personality, 50(10), 15-28.
  24. Csikszentmihalyi, M., Larson, R., & Prescott, S. (1977). The ecology of adolescent activity and experience. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 6(3), 281-294.
  25. Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Nakamura, J. (2011). Positive psychology: Where did it come from, and where is it going? In K. M. Sheldon, T. B. Kashdan, & M. F. Steger (Eds.), Designing positive psychology: Taking stock and moving forward (pp. 3-8). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  26. Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Rathunde, K. (1990). The psychology of wisdom: An evolutionary interpretation. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Wisdom: Its nature, origins, and development (pp. 25-51). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
  27. Curhan, K. B., Sims, T., Markus, H. R., Kitayama, S., Karasawa, M., Kawakami, N., … & Ryff, C. D. (2014). Just how bad negative affect is for your health depends on culture. Psychological Science, 25(12), 2277-2280.
  28. Davis, C. G., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Larson, J. (1998). Making sense of loss and benefiting from the experience: Two construals of meaning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(2), 561-574.
  29. Diener, E. (1984). Subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 95(3), 542-575.
  30. Diener, E. (2000). Subjective well-being. The science of happiness and a proposal for a national index. American Psychologist, 55(1), 34-43.
  31. Diener, E. (2008). Ed Diener: One happy autobiography. In R. Levine, L. Zelezny, and A. Rodriques (Eds.), Journeys in social psychology (pp. 1-18). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
  32. Diener, E., & Diener, C. (2011). Monitoring psychosocial prosperity for social change. In R. Biswas-Diener (Ed.), Positive psychology as social change (pp. 53-72). Dordrecht, NL: Springer.
  33. Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The Satisfaction with Life Scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49(1), 71-75.
  34. Diener, E., Heinstzelman, S. J., Kushlev, K., Tay, L., Wirtz, D., Lutes, L. D., & Oishi, S. (in press). Findings all psychologists should know from the new science on subjective well-being. Canadian Psychology.
  35. Diener, E., Lucas, R., Schimmak, U., & Helliwell, J. (2009). Well-being for public policy. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  36. Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Park, J. (2014). An incomplete list of eminent psychologists of the modern era. Archives of Scientific Psychology, 2(1), 20-31.
  37. Diener, E. & Suh, E. M. (Eds.). (2000). Culture and subjective well-being. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  38. Ehrenreich, B. (2010). Smile or die: How positive thinking fooled America and the world. London, UK: Granta.
  39. Eidelson, R., Lyubansky, M., & Malley-Morrison, K. (2014, November 18). Building a racially just society: Psychological insights. Positive Psychology. Retrieved from http://www.positivepsychology.net/articles/Building-a-Racially-Just-S-by-Roy-Eidelson-Injustice_Law-Enforcement_Psychology_Race-Racism-141118-40.html
  40. Emmons, R. A. (1986) Personal strivings: An approach to personality and subjective well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(5), 1058-1068.
  41. Emmons, R. A. (1999). The psychology of ultimate concerns: Motivation and spirituality in personality. New York, NY: Guildford.
  42. Emmons, R. A. (2005). Striving for the sacred: Personal goals, life meaning, and religion. Journal of Social Issues, 61(4), 731-745.
  43. Emmons, R. A. (2007). THANKS! How the new science of gratitude can make you happier. Boston, MA: Houghton-Mifflin.
  44. Emmons, R. A. (2010, November 16). Why gratitude is good. Greater Good Science Center. Retrieved from http://greatergood.berkeley.edu/article/item/why_gratitude_is_good
  45. Emmons, R. A. (2013) Gratitude works! A twenty-one-day program for creating emotional prosperity. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
  46. Emmons, R. A. (2016). Home. Emmons Lab. Retrieved from http://emmons.faculty.ucdavis.edu
  47. Emmons, R. A. & Hill, J. (2001). Words of gratitude for mind, body, and soul. Radnor, PA: Templeton Foundation Press.
  48. Frankl, V. E. (1985). Man’s search for meaning (Revised & updated ed.). New York, NY: Washington Square Press
  49. Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review of General Psychology, 2(3), 300-319.
  50. Fredrickson, B. L. (2003). The value of positive emotions: The emerging science of positive psychology is coming to understand why it’s good to feel good. American Scientist, 91(4), 330-335.
  51. Fredrickson, B. L. (2009). Positivity: Top-notch research reveals the 3 to 1 ratio that will change your life. New York, NY: Three Rivers Press.
  52. Fredrickson, B. L., & Branigan, C. (2005). Positive emotions broaden the scope of attention and thought-action repertoires. Cognition and Emotion, 19(3), 313-332.
  53. Fredrickson, B. L., Grewen, K. M., Coffey, K. A., Algoe, S. B., Firestine, A. M., Arevalo, J. M., … & Cole, S. W. (2013). A functional genomic perspective on human well-being. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(33), 13684-13689.
  54. Fredrickson, B. L., & Levenson, R. W. (1998). Positive emotions speed recovery from the cardiovascular sequelae of negative emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 12(2), 1-25.
  55. Fredrickson, B. L., & Losada, M. (2005). Positive affect and the complex dynamic of human flourishing. American Psychologist, 60(7), 678-686. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.60.7.678.Positive
  56. Garland, E. L., Farb, N. A., Goldin, P. R., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2015). Mindfulness broadens awareness and builds eudaimonic meaning: A process model of mindful positive emotion regulation. Psychological Inquiry, 26(4), 293-314.
  57. Getzels, J., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1969). Aesthetic opinion: An empirical study. Public Opinion Quarterly, 33(1), 34-45.
  58. Froh, J. J. (2004). The history of positive psychology: Truth be told. NYS Psychologist, 16(3), 18-20.
  59. Harris, R. (2008). The happiness trap: How to stop struggling and start living: A guide to ACT. London, UK: Constable & Robinson.
  60. Hart, K. E., & Sasso, T. (2011). Mapping the contours of contemporary positive psychology. Canadian Psychology, 52(2), 82-92.
  61. Heathers, J. A. J., Brown, N. J. L., Coyne, J. C., & Friedman, H. L. (2015). The elusory upward spiral: A reanalysis of Kok et al. (2013). Psychological Science, 26(7), 1140-1143.
  62. Huang, Y.-W., Wang, P.-S., & Lin, C.-C. (2011). Developing the curriculum and instruction model for suicide prevention and life education in Taiwan. In T. Muto, E. W. Nam, T. Nakahara (Eds.), Asian perspectives and evidence on health promotion and education. Tokyo, Japan: Springer.
  63. Huta, V. (2015). The complementary roles of eudaimonia and hedonia and how they can be pursued in practice. In S. Joseph (Ed.), Positive psychology in practice: Promoting human flourishing in work, health, education, and everyday life (2nd ed., pp. 159-184). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
  64. Huta, V. (2016a). Veronika Huta. In A. Jarden, G. Slemp, A. Chia, E. Lahti, & E. Hwang (Eds.), Positive psychologists on positive psychology (3rd Vol., pp. 114-122). Retrieved from http://www.workonwellbeing.com/pponpp3/files/Positive%20Psychologists%20on%20Positive%20Psychology%203rd%20Vol.pdf
  65. Huta, V. (2016b). Eudaimonic and hedonic orientations: Theoretical considerations and research findings. In J. Vittersø (Ed.), Handbook of eudaimonic well-being (ch. 15). New York, NY: Springer.
  66. Huta, V. (2016c). An overview of hedonic and eudaimonic well-being concepts. In L. Reinecke & M. B. Oliver (Eds.), The Routledge handbook of media use and well-being: International perspectives on theory and research on positive media effects (pp. 14-33). New York, NY: Routledge.
  67. Huta, V. (2016d). Meaning as a subjective experience. Journal of Constructivist Psychology: Meaning Summit Special Issue. doi:10.1080/10720537.2015.1119088
  68. Huta, V. (2016e, July 29). Eudaimonia as a meaningful way of living: Research on well-being, social impact, cognition, physiology, and worldviews. Invited talk presented at the 9th Biennial International Meaning Conference, Toronto, ON.
  69. Huta, V., & Ryan, R. M. (2010). Pursuing pleasure or virtue: The differential and overlapping well-being benefits of hedonic and eudaimonic motives. Journal of Happiness Studies, 11(6), 735-762. doi:10.1007/s10902-009-9171-4
  70. Huta, V. & Waterman, A. S. (2014). Eudaimonia and its distinction from hedonia: Developing a classification and terminology for understanding conceptual and operational definitions. Journal of Happiness Studies, 15, 1425-1456. doi:10.1007/s10902-013-9485-0
  71. International Network on Personal Meaning. (2012). Lifetime achievement awards. Meaning Conference. Retrieved from http://meaning.ca/conference/lifetime-achievement-awards/
  72. Ivtzan, I., Lomas, T., Hefferon, K., & Worth, P. (2016). Second wave positive psychology: Embracing the dark side of life. London, UK: Routledge.
  73. James, W. (1912). Essays in radical empiricism. New York, NY: Longmans-Green.
  74. Kim, U., Yang, K.-S., Hwang, K.-K. (Eds.) (2006). Indigenous and cultural psychology: Understanding people in context. New York, NY: Springer.
  75. Kok, B. E., Coffey, K. A., Cohn, M. A., Catalino, L. I., Vacharkulksemsuk, T., Algoe, S. B., … Fredrickson, B. L. (2013). How positive emotions build physical health: Perceived positive social connections account for the upward spiral between positive emotions and vagal tone. Psychological Science, 24(7), 1123-1132. doi:10.1177/0956797612470827
  76. Kok, B. E., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2015). Evidence for the upward spiral stands steady: A response to Heathers, Brown, Coyne, and Friedman (2015). Psychological Science, 26(7), 1144-1146.
  77. Kok, B. E., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2016). Corrigendum to “Upward spirals of the heart: Autonomic flexibility, as indexed by vagal tone, reciprocally and prospectively predicts positive emotions and social connectedness”. Biological Psychology, 117, 240. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.03.001
  78. Koltko-Rivera, M. E. (2004). The psychology of worldviews. Review of General Psychology, 8, 3-58.
  79. Koltko-Rivera, M. E. (2006). Rediscovering the later version of Maslow’s hierarchy of needs: Self-transcendence and opportunities for theory, research, and unification. Review of General Psychology, 10(4), 302-317.
  80. Kubey, R., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2002). Television addiction is no metaphor. Scientific American, 286(2), 74–80.
  81. Larson, R., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1978). Experiential correlates of time alone in adolescence. Journal of Personality, 46(4), 677-693.
  82. Larson, R., Csikszentmihalyi, M. & Graef, R. J. (1980). Mood variability and the psychosocial adjustment of adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 9(6), 469-490.
  83. Lazarus, R. S. (2003). Does the positive psychology movement have legs? Psychological Inquiry, 14(2), 93-109.
  84. Lin, Y. (1935). My country and my people. New York, NY: John Day.
  85. Linley, P. A., & Maltby, B. (2009). Personal responsibility. In S. J. Lopez (Ed.), The encyclopedia of positive psychology (pp. 685-689). Boston, MA: Blackwell.
  86. Leong, F. T. L., & Wong, P. T. P. (2003). Optimal functioning from cross-cultural perspectives. In B. Walsh (Ed.), Counseling psychology and optimal human functioning (pp. 123-150). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  87. Lyubomirsky, S. (2001). Why are some people happier than others? American Psychologist, 56(3), 239-249. doi:1O.1O37//0OO3-O66X.56.3.239
  88. Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). The how of happiness: A new approach to getting the life you want. New York, NY: Penguin Press.
  89. Lyubomirsky, S. (2013). The myths of happiness: What should make you happy, but doesn’t, what shouldn’t make you happy, but does. New York, NY: Penguin.
  90. Lyubomirsky, S., Dickerhoof, R., Boehm, J. K., & Sheldon, K. M. (2011). Becoming happier takes both a will and a proper way: An experimental longitudinal intervention to boost well-being. Emotion, 11(2), 391-402. doi:10.1037/a0022575
  91. Lyubomirsky, S., & Layous, K. (2013). How do simple positive activities increase well-being? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(1), 57-62. doi:10.1177/0963721412469809
  92. Lyubomirsky, S., & Lepper, H. (1999). A measure of subjective happiness: Preliminary reliability and construct validation. Social Indicators Research, 46(2), 137-155.
  93. Lyubomirsky, S., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1993). Self-perpetuating properties of dysphoric rumination. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(2), 339-349. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.65.2.339
  94. Lyubomirsky, S., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1995). Effects of self-focused rumination on negative thinking and interpersonal problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(1), 176-190.
  95. Martin, M. W. J. (2008). Paradoxes of happiness. Journal of Happiness Studies, 9(2), 171-184. doi:10.1007/s10902-007-9056-3
  96. Maslow, A. H. (1943). A theory of human motivation. Psychological Review, 50(4), 370-396.
  97. Maslow, A. H. (1954). Motivation and personality (1st ed.). New York, NY: Harper.
  98. Maslow, A. H. (1961). Existential psychology: What’s in it for us? In R. May (Ed.), Existential psychology (pp. 49-57). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
  99. Maslow, A. H. (1970). Motivation and personality (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Harper & Row.
  100. Maslow, A. H. (1971). The farther reaches of human nature. New York, NY: Viking.
  101. Maslow, A. H. (2002). The psychology of science: A reconnaissance. Chapel Hill, NC: Bassett. (Original 1966)
  102. McCormick, R., & Wong, P. T. P. (2006). Adjustment and coping in aboriginal people. In P. T. P. Wong, & L. C. J. Wong (Eds.), Handbook of multicultural perspectives on stress and coping (pp. 515-531). New York, NY: Springer.
  103. McDonald, M. J., Wong, P. T. P., & Gingras, D. T. (2012). Meaning-in-life measures and development of a brief version of the Personal Meaning Profile. In P. T. P. Wong (Ed.), The human quest for meaning: Theories, research, and applications (2nd ed., pp. 357-382). New York, NY: Routledge.
  104. McGrath, R. E. (2015). Character strengths in 75 nations: An update. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 10(1), 41–52. doi:10.1080/17439760.2014.888580
  105. Miller, G. A. (1969). Psychology as a means of promoting human welfare. American Psychologist, 24(23), 1063-1075.
  106. Oettingen, G. (2014). Rethinking positive thinking: Inside the new science of motivation. New York, NY: Penguin Random House.
  107. Park, N., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2013). Christopher M. Peterson (1950-2012). American Psychologist, 68(5), 403. doi:10.1037/a0033380
  108. Parks, A. C., & Biswas-Diener, R. (2013). Positive interventions: Past, present, and future. In T. Kashdan & J. Ciarrochi (Eds.), Mindfulness, acceptance, and positive psychology: The seven foundations of well-being (pp. 140-165). Oakland, CA: New Harbinger.
  109. Patterson, T. G., & Joseph, S. (2007). Person-centered personality theory: Support from self-determination theory and positive psychology. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 47(1), 117-139.
  110. Peacock, E. J., & Wong, P. T. P. (1990). The Stress Appraisal Measure (SAM): A multidimensional approach to cognitive appraisalStress Medicine, 6(3), 227-236.
  111. Peterson, C. (2006). A primer in positive psychology. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  112. Peterson, C., Park, N., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2005). Orientations to happiness and life satisfaction: The full life versus the empty life. Journal of Happiness Studies, 6(1), 25–41. doi:10.1007/s10902-004-1278-z
  113. Peterson, C., Park, N., Pole, N., D’Andrea, W., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2008). Strengths of character and posttraumatic growth. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 21(2), 214–217. doi:10.1002/jts.
  114. Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2004). Character strengths and virtues: A handbook and classification. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
  115. Peterson, C., Semmel, A., von Baeyer, C., Abramson, L. Y., Metalsky, G. I., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1982). The Attributional Style Questionnaire. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 6(3), 287-299.
  116. Peterson, C., & Villanova, P. (1988). An expanded Attributional Style Questionnaire. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97(1), 87-89. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.97.1.87
  117. Peterson, C., & Park, N. (2014). Meaning and positive psychology. International Journal of Existential Psychology and Psychotherapy, 5(1), 2-8.
  118. Proctor, C., Tweed, R., & Morris, D. (2015). The naturally emerging structure of well-being among young adults: “Big Two” or other framework? Journal of Happiness Studies, 16(1), 257-275. doi:10.1007/s10902-014-9507-6
  119. Proctor, C., Tweed, R., & Morris, D. (2016). The Rogerian fully functioning person: A positive psychology perspective. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 56(5), 503-529. doi:10.1177/0022167815605936
  120. Reker, G. T., Peacock, E. J., & Wong, P. T. P. (1987). Meaning and purpose in life and well-being: A life-span perspective. Journal of Gerontology, 42(1), 44-49.
  121. Reker, G. T., & Wong, P. T. P. (1988). Aging as an individual process: Towards a theory of personal meaning. In J. E. Birren & V. L. Bengston (Eds.), Emergent theories of aging (pp. 214-246). New York, NY: Springer.
  122. Reker, G. T., & Wong, P. T. P. (2012). Personal meaning in life and psychosocial adaptation in the later years. In P. T. P. Wong (Ed.), The human quest for meaning: Theories, research, and applications (2nd ed., pp. 433-456). New York, NY: Routledge.
  123. Rogers, C. (1995). On becoming a person: A therapist’s view of psychotherapy. Boston, MA: Harcourt. (Original published 1961)
  124. Rosenbaum, M. E. (1990). Learned resourcefulness: On coping skills, self-control, and adaptive behavior. New York, NY: Springer.
  125. Rubin, G. (2009). The happiness project. New York, NY: Harper.
  126. Ruini, C., & Ryff, C. D. (2016). Using eudaimonic well-being to improve lives. In A. M. Wood & J. Johnson (Eds.), The Wiley handbook of positive clinical psychology: An integrative approach to studying and improving well-being. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Blackwell.
  127. Ryan, R. M., Curren, R. R., & Deci, E. L. (2013). What humans need: Flourishing in Aristotelian philosophy and self-determination theory. In A. S. Waterman (Ed.), The best within us: Positive psychology perspectives on eudaimonia (pp. 57-75). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
  128. Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55(1), 68-78.
  129. Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(6), 1069- 1081.
  130. Ryff, C. D. (2003). Corners of myopia in the positive psychology parade. Psychological Inquiry, 14(2), 153-159.
  131. Ryff, C. D. (2012). Existential well-being and health. In P. T. P. Wong (Ed.), The human quest for meaning: Theories, research, and applications (2nd ed., pp. 233-248). New York, NY: Routledge.
  132. Ryff, C. D. (2014a). Self-realisation and meaning making in the face of adversity: A eudaimonic approach to human resilience. Journal of Psychology in Africa, 24(1), 1-12.
  133. Ryff, C. D. (2014b). Psychological well-being revisited: Advances in the science and practice of eudaimonia. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 83(1), 10-28.
  134. Ryff. C. D. (2016, July 30). Self-realization and meaning making in the face of adversity: A eudaimonic approach to human resilience. Invited workshop presented at the 9th Biennial International Meaning Conference, Toronto, ON.
  135. Ryff, C. D., Boylan, J. M., Coe, C. L., Karasawa, M., Kawakami, N., Kitayama, S., … & Park, J. (2015). Adult development in Japan and the United States: Comparing theories and findings about growth, maturity, and well-being. In L. A. Jensen (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of human development and culture: An interdisciplinary perspective (pp. 666-679). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  136. Ryff, C. D., Love, G. D., Miyamoto, Y., Markus, H. R., Curhan, K. B., Kitayama, S., … & Karasawa, M. (2014). Culture and the promotion of well-being in East and West: Understanding varieties of attunement to the surrounding context. In G. A. Fava & C. Ruini (Eds.), Increasing psychological well-being in clinical and educational settings (pp. 1-19). New York, NY: Springer.
  137. Ryff, C. D. & Singer, B. H. (1998). The role of purpose in life and personal growth in positive human health. In P. T. P. Wong & P. S. Fry (Eds.), The human quest for meaning: A handbook of psychological research and clinical applications (pp. 213-235). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  138. Ryff, C. D., & Singer, B. H. (2008). Know thyself and become what you are: A eudaimonic approach to psychological well-being. Journal of Happiness Studies, 9(1), 13-39.
  139. Schnell, T. (2009). The Source of Meaning and Meaning in Life Questionnaire (SoMe): Relations to demographics and well-being. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 4(6), 483-499.
  140. Schwartz, B., & Sharpe, K. (2010). Practical wisdom: The right way to do the right thing. New York, NY: Riverhead Books.
  141. Scott, W. A., & Peterson, C. (1975). Adjustment, Pollyannaism, and attraction to close relationships. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 43(6), 872-880.
  142. Seifert, T. A. (2005, Spring). The Ryff Scales of Psychological Well-Being. Center of Inquiry at Wabash College. Retrieved from http://www.liberalarts.wabash.edu/ryff-scales/
  143. Seligman, M. E. P. (1975). Helplessness: On depression, development, and death. San Francisco, CA: Freeman.
  144. Seligman, M. E. P. (1990). Learned optimism: How to change your mind and your life. New York, NY: Pocket Books.
  145. Seligman, M. E. P. (1995). The optimistic child: How learned optimism protects children from depression. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.
  146. Seligman, M. E. P. (1998). Building human strength: Psychology’s forgotten mission. APA Monitor, 29(1), 2.
  147. Seligman, M. E. P. (September, 1999a). Summit 1999 speech by Martin Seligman. Presented at the Positive Psychology Summit, Lincoln, NE. Retrieved from https://ppc.sas.upenn.edu/opportunities/conference-archives
  148. Seligman, M. E. P. (1999b). Positive psychology network concept paper. Unpublished manuscript. Retrieved from https://ppc.sas.upenn.edu/sites/ppc.sas.upenn.edu/files/PP%20Network%20Concept%20Paper.docx
  149. Seligman, M. E. P. (2002). Authentic happiness: Using the new positive psychology to realize your potential for lasting fulfillment. New York, NY: Free Press.
  150. Seligman, M. E. P. (2011). Flourish: A visionary new understanding of happiness and well-being. New York, NY: Free Press.
  151. Seligman, M. E. P., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive psychology: An introduction. American Psychologist, 55(1), 5-14.
  152. Seligman, M. E. P., Ernst, R. M., Gillham, J., Reivich, K., & Linkins, M. (2009). Positive education: Positive psychology and classroom interventions. Oxford Review of Education, 35(3), 293-311.
  153. Seligman, M. E. P., & Maier, S. F. (1967). Failure to escape traumatic shock. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47(1), 1-9. http://doi.org/10.1037/h0024514
  154. Sheldon, K., Fredrickson, B. L., Rathunde, K. & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive psychology manifesto. Unpublished manuscript. Retrieved from https://ppc.sas.upenn.edu/sites/ppc.sas…/Positive%20Psychology%20Manifesto.docx
  155. Smith, E. E. (2017). The power of meaning: Crafting a life that matters. New York, NY: Crown.
  156. Steger, M. F., Kashdan, T. B., & Oishi, S. (2008). Being good by doing good: Daily eudaimonic activity and well-being. Journal of Research in Personality, 42(1), 22-42. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2007.03.004
  157. Su, R., Tay, L., & Diener, E. (2014). The development and validation of the Comprehensive Inventory of Thriving (CIT) and the Brief Inventory of Thriving (BIT). Applied Psychology: Health and Well-Being, 6(3), 251-279. doi:10.1111/aphw.12027
  158. Taubes, G. (2010, August). Richard Ryan on the many applications of Self-Determination Theory. Science Watch. Retrieved from http://archive.sciencewatch.com/inter/aut/2010/10-aug/10augRyan/
  159. Rashid, T., & Howes, R. N. (2016). Positive psychotherapy: Clinical application of positive psychology. In A. M. Wood & J. Johnson (Eds.), The Wiley handbook of positive clinical psychology (pp. 321-348). New York, NY: Wiley.
  160. Tay, L., & Diener, E. (2011). Needs and subjective well-being around the world. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101(2), 354-365.
  161. Templeton, J. M. (1998). The humble approach: Scientists discover God. West Conshohocken, PA: Templeton Press.
  162. Thomson Reuters. (2015, July). H-index indicator for Richard M. Ryan. Web of Science.
  163. Tweed, R. G., Biswas-Diener, R., & Lehman, D. R. (2012). Self-perceived strengths among people who are homeless. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 7(6), 481-492.
  164. Tweed, R. G., White, K., & Lehman, D. R. (2004) Culture, stress, and coping: Internally- and externally-targeted control strategies of European Canadians, East Asian-Canadians, and Japanese. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 35(6), 652-658.
  165. Ujimoto, K. V., Nishio, H. K., Wong, P. T. P., & Lam, L. (1993). Cultural factors affecting the self-assessment of health satisfaction. In R. Masi, L. Mensah, & K. McLeod (Eds.), Health and culture: Explaining the relationships (pp. 229-240). Toronto, ON: Canadian Council on Multicultural Health.
  166. Vallerand, R. J. (2015). The psychology of passion: A dualistic model. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  167. van Cappellen, P., May, B. W., Isgett, S. F., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2016). Effects of oxytocin administration on spirituality and emotional responses to meditation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(10), 1579-1587.
  168. Vansteenkiste, M., & Ryan, R.M. (2013). On psychological growth and vulnerability: Basic psychological need satisfaction and need frustration as a unifying principle. Journal of Psychotherapy Integration, 23(3), 263-280.
  169. Vela, J. C., Lu, M. T. P., Lenz, A. S., & Hinojosa, K. (2015). Positive psychology and familial factors as predictors of Latina/o students’ psychological grit. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 37(3), 287-303.
  170. Weiss, L. & Hickman, S. (Eds.). (n.d.). What is mindfulness? Greater Good Science Center. Retrieved from http://greatergood.berkeley.edu/topic/mindfulness/definition
  171. Williams, M. & Penman, D. (2012). Mindfulness: An eight-week plan for finding peace in a frantic world. New York, NY: Rodale.
  172. Wong, P. T. P. (1977). A behavioral field approach to instrumental learning in the rat: I. Partial reinforcement effects and sex differences. Animal Learning & Behavior, 5(1), 5-13.
  173. Wong, P. T. P. (1979). Frustration, exploration, and learningCanadian Psychological Review, 20, 133-144.
  174. Wong, P. T. P. (1989). Personal meaning and successful aging. Canadian Psychology, 30, 516-525.
  175. Wong, P. T. P. (1991). Existential vs. causal attributions: The social perceiver as philosopher. In S. L. Zelen (Ed.), New models, new extensions of attribution theory (pp. 84-125). New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.
  176. Wong, P. T. P. (1993). Effective management of life stress: The resource-congruence model. Stress Medicine, 9, 51-60.
  177. Wong, P. T. P. (1995). A stage model of coping with frustrative stress. In R. Wong (Ed.), Biological perspectives on motivated activities (pp. 339-378). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
  178. Wong, P. T. P. (1998a). Implicit theories of meaningful life and the development of the Personal Meaning Profile (PMP). In P. T. P. Wong, & P. Fry (Eds.), The human quest for meaning: A handbook of psychological research and clinical applications (pp. 111-140). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  179. Wong, P. T. P. (1998b). Academic values and achievement motivation. In P. T. P. Wong, & P. Fry (Eds.), The human quest for meaning: A handbook of psychological research and clinical applications (pp. 261-292). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  180. Wong, P. T. P. (2005). Existential and humanistic theories. In J. C. Thomas & D. L. Segal (Eds.), Comprehensive handbook of personality and psychopathology (pp. 192-211). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
  181. Wong, P. T. P. (2006a). The positive psychology of persistence and flexibility. Positive Living Newsletter. Retrieved from http://meaning.ca/archives/presidents_columns/pres_col_feb_2006_persistance-and-flexibility.htm
  182. Wong, P. T. P. (2006b). The nature and practice of compassion: Integrating Western and Eastern positive psychologies [Review of the book Compassion: Conceptualizations, research, and use in psychotherapy]. PsycCRITIQUES, 51(25). doi:10.1037/a0002884
  183. Wong, P. T. P. (2007). Introduction: A quiet positive revolution. In P. T. P. Wong, L. C. J. Wong, M. J. McDonald, & D. W. Klaassen (Eds.), The positive psychology of meaning and spirituality: Selected papers from meaning conferences (pp. 1-8). Abbotsford, BC: INPM Press.
  184. Wong, P. T. P. (2009a). Existential positive psychology. In S. J. Lopez (Ed.), Encyclopedia of positive psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 361-368). Oxford, UK: Wiley Blackwell.
  185. Wong, P. T. P. (2009b). Chinese positive psychology. In S. J. Lopez (Ed.), Encyclopedia of positive psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 148-156). Oxford, UK: Wiley Blackwell.
  186. Wong, P. T. P. (2009c). Viktor Frankl: Prophet of hope for the 21st century. In A. Batthyany, & J. Levinson (Eds.), Existential psychotherapy of meaning: Handbook of logotherapy and existential analysis. Phoenix, AZ: Zeig, Tucker & Theisen.
  187. Wong, P. T. P. (2010). Meaning therapy: An integrative and positive existential psychotherapyJournal of Contemporary Psychotherapy, 40(2), 85-99.
  188. Wong, P. T. P. (2011a). Big money, big science, big names, and the flourishing of positive psychology [Review of the book Flourish: A visionary new understanding of happiness and well-being]. PsycCRITIQUES, 56(49). doi:10.1037/a0026281
  189. Wong, P. T. P. (2011b). Reclaiming positive psychology: A meaning-centered approach to sustainable growth and radical empiricismJournal of Humanistic Psychology, 51(4), 408-412.
  190. Wong, P. T. P. (2011c). Positive psychology 2.0: Towards a balanced interactive model of the good life. Canadian Psychology, 52(2), 69-81.
  191. Wong, P. T. P. (2012a). Toward a dual-systems model of what makes life worth living. In P. T. P. Wong (Ed.), The human quest for meaning: Theories, research, and applications (2nd ed., pp. 3-22). New York, NY: Routledge.
  192. Wong, P. T. P. (2012b). What is the meaning mindset? International Journal of Existential Psychology and Psychotherapy, 4(1), 1-3.
  193. Wong, P. T. P. (2012c). The human quest for meaning: Theories, research, and applications (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Routledge.
  194. Wong, P. T. P. (2012d, Jun. 22). Giving positive psychology away: Meaningful living meetups. Positive Psychology News Daily. Retrieved from http://positivepsychologynews.com/news/paul-wong/2012062222849
  195. Wong, P. T. P. (2013a). Paul Wong. In A. Jarden, M. M. O. Bruna, E. Lahti, & Y. Zhao (Eds.), Positive psychologists on positive psychology (Vol. 2.; pp. 113-118). Retrieved from https://www.workonwellbeing.com/pponpp3/files/Positive%20Psychologists%20on%20Positive%20Psychology%202nd%20Vol.pdf
  196. Wong, P. T. P. (2013b). Positive psychology. In K. Keith (Ed.), Encyclopedia of cross-cultural psychology. Oxford, UK: Wiley Blackwell.
  197. Wong, P. T. P. (2013c, October). A meaning-centered approach to positive education. Invited lecture presented at FoGuang University, Yilan County, Taiwan.
  198. Wong, P. T. P. (2013d). Suicide risks among college students from diverse cultural backgrounds. Directions in Psychiatry, 33(4), 237-249.
  199. Wong, P. T. P. (2014). Viktor Frankl’s meaning seeking model and positive psychology. In A. Batthyany & P. Russo-Netzer (Eds.), Meaning in existential and positive psychology (pp. 149-184). New York, NY: Springer.
  200. Wong, P. T. P. (2015). Meaning therapy: Assessments and interventionsExistential Analysis, 26(1), 154-167.
  201. Wong, P. T. P. (2016a). Chinese positive psychology revisitedInternational Journal of Existential Psychology and Psychotherapy, 6(1). Retrieved from http://www.drpaulwong.com/chinese-positive-psychology-revisited/
  202. Wong, P. T. P. (2016b). Self-transcendence: A paradoxical way to become your bestInternational Journal of Existential Psychology and Psychotherapy, 6(1). Retrieved from http://www.drpaulwong.com/wp-content/uploads/2016/03/Self-Transcendence_A-Paradoxical-Way-to-Become-Your-Best-2016-Aug-15.pdf
  203. Wong, P. T. P. (2016c). Integrative meaning therapy: From logotherapy to existential positive interventions. In P. Russo-Netzer, S. E. Schulenberg, & A. Batthyany (Eds.). Clinical perspectives on meaning: Positive and existential psychotherapy. New York, NY: Springer.
  204. Wong, P. T. P. (2016d). Autobiography. Dr. Paul Wong. Retrieved from http://www.drpaulwong.com/writing/autobiography/
  205. Wong, P. T. P. (2016e, June). Self-transcendence as the path to virtue, happiness and meaning Paper presented at the research working group meeting for Virtue, Happiness, and the Meaning of Life Project, University of Chicago, Chicago, Illinois. (Funded by the John Templeton Foundation)
  206. Wong, P. T. P. (2016f, July). Working with MIL in meaning therapy. Presented in a symposium on “Working with MIL issues” at the 9th Biennial International Meaning Conference, Toronto, ON.
  207. Wong, P. T. P. (2016g). Meaning centered positive group intervention. In P. Russo-Netzer, S. Schulenberg, & A. Batthyány (Eds.), Clinical perspectives on meaning: Positive and existential psychotherapy. New York, NY: Springer.
  208. Wong, P. T. P. (2016h, September). Mapping the contour of second wave positive psychology (PP 2.0). Positive Living Newsletter. Retrieved from http://www.drpaulwong.com/inpm-presidents-report-september-2016/
  209. Wong, P. T. P. (in press-a). Creativity and giftedness from the perspective of second wave positive psychology (PP 2.0). Psychology and Education.
  210. Wong, P. T. P. (in press-b). Meaning-centered approach to research and therapy, second wave positive psychology, and the future of humanistic psychology. The Humanistic Psychologist.
  211. Wong, P. T. P., Ivtzan, I., & Lomas, T. (2016). Good work: A meaning-centered approach. In L. G. Oades, M. F. Steger, A. Delle Fave, & J. Passmore (Eds.), The Wiley Blackwell handbook of the psychology of positivity and strengths-based approaches at work (pp. 0-0). West Sussex, UK: Wiley Blackwell.
  212. Wong, P. T. P., & McDonald, M. (2002). Tragic optimism and personal meaning in counselling victims of abusePastoral Sciences, 20(2), 231-249.
  213. Wong, P. T. P., Reker, G. T., & Gesser, G. (1994). Death Attitude Profile – Revised: A multidimensional measure of attitudes toward death. In R. A. Neimeyer (Ed.), Death anxiety handbook: Research, instrumentation, and application (pp. 121-148). Washington, DC: Taylor & Francis.
  214. Wong, P. T. P., Reker, G. T. & Peacock, E. (2006). The resource-congruence model of coping and the development of the Coping Schema Inventory. In P. T. P. Wong, & L. C. J., Wong (Eds.), Handbook of multicultural perspectives on stress and coping (pp. 223-283). New York, NY: Springer.
  215. Wong, P. T. P., & Roy, S. (in press). Critique of positive psychology and positive interventions. In N. J. L. Brown, T. Lomas, & F. J. Eiroa-Orosa (Eds.), The Routledge international handbook of critical positive psychology. London, UK: Routledge.
  216. Wong, P. T. P., & Sproule, C. F. (1984). Attributional analysis of locus of control and the Trent Attribution Profile (TAP). In H. M. Lefcourt (Ed.), Research with the locus of control construct: Limitations and extensions (Vol. 3; pp. 309-360). New York, NY: Academic Press.
  217. Wong, P. T. P., & Ujimoto, K. V. (1998). The elderly: Their stress, coping, and mental health. In L. C. Lee, & N. W. S. Zane (Eds.), Handbook of Asian American psychology (pp. 165-209). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
  218. Wong, P. T. P., & Watt, L. (1991). What types of reminiscence are associated with successful aging? Psychology and Aging, 6, 272-279.
  219. Wong, P. T. P., & Weiner, B. (1981). When people ask “Why” questions and the heuristic of attributional search. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 650-663.
  220. Wong, P. T. P., & Wong, L. C. J. (Eds.). (2006). Handbook of multicultural perspectives on stress and coping. New York, NY: Springer.
  221. Wong, P. T. P., Wong, L. C. J., & Scott, C. (2006). Beyond stress and coping: The positive psychology of transformation. In Wong, P. T. P., & Wong, L. C. J. (Eds.), Handbook of multicultural perspectives on stress and coping (pp. 1-26). New York, NY: Springer.
  222. Wright, R. (2012, Oct. 14). Very nice guy (and important psychologist) dies. The Atlantic. Retrieved from http://www.theatlantic.com/health/archive/2012/10/very-nice-guy-and-important-psychologist-dies/263595/
  223. Yalom, I. D. (1980). Existential psychotherapy. New York, NY: BasicBooks.

A shorter version will be published as Wong, P. T. P., & Tweed, R. (in press). Positive psychology in North America. In E. C. Chang, C. A. Downey, H. Yang, I. Zettler, & M. Muyan (Eds.), The international handbook of positive psychology: A global perspective on the science of positive human existence. New York, NY: Springer.