Abstract

This chapter argues that since Chinese culture is complex and profound, uncritical transplant of American positive psychology to Chinese soil may not be fruitful. It proposes that a more promising approach to Chinese Positive Psychology (CPP) calls for research programs that meet the needs of the Chinese people in their unique cultural and political context. More specifically, it first describes the defining characteristics of the Chinese culture and then outlines three related tracks of research: (1) Basic psychological research based on the Chinese yin-yang dialectics (2) The indigenous psychology movement; and (3) Cross-cultural psychology research. This paper concludes that Wong’s (2011) dialectical perspective of Positive Psychology 2.0 may provide a more culturally appropriate framework for a productive CPP and a hopeful future for China.

Introduction

Chinese Positive Psychology (CPP) has gained much momentum as a result of several current trends. First, China’s recent unprecedented economic growth and political influence in the world has generated a great deal of interest in the psychology of Chinese people (Bond, 2010). Second, the psychology department in Tsinghua University, re-established by K. Peng, focuses on positive psychology (Kuhn, 2013). Third, two international positive psychology conferences (2010, 2012) have been held in China in 2010 and 2012. Finally, there is also a state-sponsored happiness campaign in China (J. Yang, 2014).

While Wong (2010) affirmed the potential benefits of PP for China, he also cautioned against the uncritical and wholesale transplant of American positive psychology to Chinese soil for two main reasons. First, American PP had its own inherent limitations, as pointed by various scholars (e.g., Wong, 2011). Second, what works in America might not work in China and vice versa; for positive psychology to take roots in China, it needs to be culturally sensitive.

In a similar vein, Jie Yang (2014) pointed out that the millions of marginalized and displaced urban workers in China, resulting from national economic restructuring, did not have the necessary resources and skills to create their own jobs and reconstruct their own happiness. In other words, a state-sponsored happiness campaign that focuses on individual responsibility, without the government providing job opportunities and mental health services, may have the opposite effect of generating resentment towards the State.

A stronger case against the uncritical transplant of American PP is that PP is inherently culture-bound (Wong, 2013). What is happiness or what constitutes a good life depends on a priori value judgments based on cultural norms (Ho, Duan, & Tang, 2014; Lu, 2008, 2010). The experience of what is meaningful is also grounded in the historical and cultural context (Zevnik, 2014). This chapter argues that a more fruitful approach to CPP is to develop research methodologies that are sensitive to traditional Chinese cultural values and incorporate the best from Western psychology and Chinese indigenous psychology. 

Key Characteristics of the Chinese Cultural Values

Culture is a multidimensional construct that encompasses people, history, geography, politics, customs, language, and influential philosophies and religions. The accumulated wisdom from historical lessons and the teachings of Confucianism, Taoism, and Buddhism have laid the foundation for Chinese mentality.

Three Dominant Chinese Philosophies

Confucianism. Confucianism is most responsible for shaping Chinese culture. Witnessing the decay of social order and escalation of wars, Confucius sought to realize the ideal of social stability and world peace through the cultivation of individual virtues. Confucius taught that educated gentlemen should practice the following five virtues in order to live a happy and harmonious life: Yen (benevolence), Yi (righteousness). Li (propriety), Zhi (wisdom) and Xin (integrity). In sum, the good life consists of finding one’s proper place in society and dutifully performing one’s role according to the above 5 cardinal values. Such emphasis on collectivism may be responsible for holding the country together throughout its long history.

Taoism. Taoism advocates the ideal of returning to the simple and natural way of life as a way of coping with the hardships and uncertainties of life. It was founded by Lao Tze and elaborated by Zhung Tze. The word Tao literally means “the way” or “the way of nature”—the ultimate creative principle that gives birth to the universe and nourishes everything in the cosmos.

One of the profound insights of Lao Tze is the duality of nature. All things in nature exist in duality or dialectics. The two opposites complement each other and make the existence of each other possible. Goodness does not exist without evil. Happiness does not exist without unhappiness. Lao Tze observes: “Fortune owes its existence to misfortune, and misfortune is hidden in fortune” (quoted by Chen, 2006, p. 92).

Taoism teaches contentment as a natural way of life. It teaches us not only how to be free from worries, but also how to achieve happiness even when problems are pervasive, chronic and beyond one’s control. Contentment leads to humble, selfless devotion to the well-being of humanity.

The Buddhist perspective. Buddhism was founded by Siddhartha Gautama as a system of mental cultivation in order to achieve spiritual liberation from suffering through awakening of the mind from delusion and greed. Mahayana Buddhism (great vehicle), later developed in China, stresses the ideal of Bodhisattvas – enlightened individuals who are moved by compassion to save all sentient beings from sufferings.

The basic tenets of Buddhism are the Four Noble Truths:

  1. The Truth of Suffering (Dikkha) – Life is full of suffering
  2. The Truth of the Cause of Suffering (Tanha) – Suffering comes from craving for happiness and aversion to pain.
  3. The Truth of Liberation from suffering (Nirvana) – We can be liberated from suffering by transforming our craving and aversion through enlightenment.
  4. The Truth of the Eightfold Path (Magga) – Liberation through enlightenment can be achieved through the eightfold path. Mindful meditation is only part of the eightfold path.

The above sketch of traditional Chinese cultural values presents a rather different view of the good life than that of American PP. The next section briefly discusses six cultural beliefs or worldviews, which are rooted in the above values and national experiences.

Cultural Beliefs or Worldviews

The following six cultural beliefs are dominant in the Chinese way of thinking: uncontrollability of the world, the ubiquity of change, fatalism, duality, collectivism, and utility of efforts.

Uncontrollability of the world. People perceive the external world as largely beyond their control. Individuals are not able to prevent or control powerful cosmic, natural and political forces that impact their lives.

The ubiquity of change. Since individuals have little or no control over most events and situations in their lives, they have no way to predict how life will turn out. The vast sweep of Chinese history further reinforces the perception that everything is in a flux and life is often characterized by reversals of fortune.

Fatalism. It is the belief that spiritual and cosmic forces are deciding the fate of individuals and their daily affairs. Since the world is uncontrollable and unpredictable from the perspective of individuals, belief in fatalism seems to be inevitable. One benefit of belief in fatalism is that it makes unexplainable adversities more bearable. When one attributes suffering to karma, fate or bad luck, then one is liberated from the bondage of shame, guilt, or anger.

The duality of nature. Emphasized by Taoism, the belief in duality recognizes the co-existence of opposites. The symbol of Yin Yang expresses best the dynamic balance between opposites in human nature as well as in the human condition. Such dualistic and dialectical thinking enables the Chinese to accommodate contradicting ideas and seek compromise. It also enables them to be tolerant of misfortunes, which may blessings in disguise.

Collectivism. Crowdedness and the enormity of life’s problems make it necessary for the Chinese people to learn how to get along with each other and how to work together to find solutions. Confucianism has also instilled into the Chinese mindset the imperative of collectivist beliefs. Loyalty to family and friends is highly valued. While the expression of gratitude is considered by American PP as an exercise to increase individual happiness, it is considered by CPP as essential for maintaining good relationships. Collective coping (Wong, 1993), which is commonly practiced in China, also stems from the culture of collectivism.

The utility of efforts. If the first five cultural beliefs make Chinese people feel helpless and powerless as individuals, belief in the utility of effort reminds them of their personal responsibility to do their very best in areas where they can exercise some control. Utility of efforts includes work ethic (being studious and conscientious), self-cultivation efforts (cultivation and accumulation of various virtues), and relational efforts (filial piety, loyalty towards friends, respect for authority, and harmony within the group). Their dialectical thinking allows the Chinese people to two contradictory beliefs simultaneously. (1) Leave everything to fate and Heaven, and (2) Sincere determination can defeat fate.

Combinations of the above traditional Chinese worldviews have served the Chinese people well in maintaining 5000 years of uninterrupted existence as a sovereign country as well as the prosperity of overseas Chinese around the globe. These worldviews should inform the development of a scientific CPP. It would be a shame for China to import American PP at the expense of her own rich cultural tradition.

Basic Psychological Research from a Chinese Perspective

The above traditional Chinese worldviews have already had a considerable impact on research in mainstream psychology. Peng & Nisbett (1999) showed that Chinese thinking is dialectical rather than logical. Similarly, the wisdom of yin-yang dialectics has informed most of Wong’s research relevant to CPP.

Wong and Sproule (1984) argued that Chinese people can hold an external and internal locus of control simultaneously. Therefore, their locus of control beliefs can only be measured by a two-dimensional space with an external and internal locus of control as two independent scales. Such dialectical thinking poses a challenge to any psychological measures that depend solely on unidimensional rating scales.

According to the same dialectical thinking, pessimism and optimism can co-exist, resulting in tragic optimism (Wong, 2009). Death fear and death acceptance can co-exist, resulting in the multidimensional Death Attitude Profile (Wong, Reker, & Gesser, 1994). Resources and deficits can co-exist, as conceptualized in the Resource-Congruence Model (Wong, 1993). Thus, family can be resources for coping, but intra-family conflict can also be a deficit or stressor (Wong & Ujimoto, 1998).

The same kind of dialectical reasoning is also behind Wong’s (2011) development of Positive Psychology 2.0, which posits that every positive emotion or experience has its downside, and every negative emotion or experience has its upside. PP 2.0 provides a dialectical, balanced, and cross-cultural perspective – a blueprint more relevant to the Chinese culture. More recently, Wong’s Dual-Systems Model (Wong, 2012) spells out the mechanisms whereby the good life can be achieved, not by accentuating the positive and avoiding the negative, but by embracing and integrating both positive and negative experiences. 

Indigenous Chinese Positive Psychology

On the basis of the above Chinese worldviews and values, it seems natural that CPP would follow a different direction. Thus, balance and moderation are valued more than achieving optimal levels of individual functioning and happiness. Group harmony is considered more important than individual success. Contentment is the key to lasting happiness. The ideal life according to the average Chinese person down through the years is: Live a simple life in peace and harmony with family members and neighbours.

Kuo-shu Yang (1988) and David Ho (1996, 1998) identified the Confucius value of filial piety as the foundation for virtue, well-being, and social harmony in China. More recently, Hwang’s (2011, 2012) research on Confucian relationalism captures the essence of the relational self in the Chinese mentality.

Sundararajan (2014), another leader in indigenous psychology, critiques American PP from a Confucian perspective. She argues that in the Chinese cultural context, negative emotions can serve as a motivational force toward the enhancement of empathy and the pursuit of virtue. Thus, in certain situations, reflection on one’s negative emotions can result in more positive outcomes than focusing only on positive emotions.

The Cross-Cultural Psychology Approach to Chinese Positive Psychology

Cross-cultural research is interested in discovering both the universal principles (etic) and culture-specific aspects (emic) of behaviour. The dominant methodologies in cross-cultural research have been critically evaluated (Sue & Sue, 1987; Zane & Sue, 1986). The assumption of universality in the constructs and assessment instruments of mainstream American psychology has been questioned. Triandis (1972) called such an ethnocentric approach ‘pseudo-etic’ rather than etic because it imposes the categories of Western culture (which is emic rather than etic) on other cultures. Such criticism is especially valid with respect to CPP.

Cross-cultural research needs to be more balanced and more sensitive to the emic difference in non-Western cultures. For example, Wong and Reker (1985) found that Chinese elderly used qualitatively different coping strategy and experienced lower psychological well-being, as compared with Anglos. K.Yang and Bond (1990) conducted research on implicit personality theories with indigenous or imported constructs in a Chinese sample. In a multi-national, cross-cultural research project, Lun and Bond (2013) employed different measures of religion and spirituality in different national contexts. Their research represents a significant improvement in cross-cultural research, as compared to using different translations of the same American constructs and measures in different nations.

Conclusion

The above review argues that while scientific research based on the positivist paradigm helps unpack the mysteries of happiness and well-being in the Euro-American context, such findings may not fit in the unique cultural and political context in China. The three related tracks of research indicate that traditional Chinese cultural beliefs have great potential to influence CPP as well as mainstream psychology. In addition to individual research programs, we may benefit from a national research agenda that systematically investigates indigenous Chinese worldviews, values, and practices that best serve the Chinese people and their national interest. Wong’s (2011) dialectical Positive Psychology 2.0 may provide a culturally appropriate conceptual framework for a more productive CPP and a hopeful future for China.

References

  1. Bond, M. H. (Ed.). (2010). The Oxford handbook of Chinese psychology. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  2. Ho, D. Y. (1996). Filial piety and its psychological consequences. In M. H. Bond (Ed.), The handbook of Chinese psychology (pp. 155-165). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  3. Ho, D. Y. F. (1998). Indigenous psychologies: Asian perspectives. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 29, 88-103.
  4. Ho, S. M. Y., Duan, W., & Tang, S. C. M. (2014). The psychology of virtue and happiness in Western and Asian thought. In N. E. Snow & F. V. Trivigno (eds.), The Philosophy and Psychology of Character and Happiness (pp. 215-238). New York, NY: Routledge.
  5. Hwang, K. K. (2011). Foundations of Chinese psychology: Confucian social relations (Vol. 1). New York, NY: Springer.
  6. Hwang, K. K. (2012). Foundations of Chinese psychology: Confucian social relations. New York, NY: Springer SBM.
  7. Kuhn, R. L. (2013, July 19). Positive pathway to follow. China Daily USA, p. 8. Retrieved from http://usa.chinadaily.com.cn/opinion/2013-07/19/content_16797180.htm
  8. Lu, L. (2008). Culture, self, and subjective well-being: Cultural psychological and social change perspectives. Psychologia51, 290-303.
  9. Lu, L. (2010). Chinese well-being. In M. H. Bond (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of Chinese psychology (pp. 327-342). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  10. Lun, V. M.-C., & Bond, M. H. (2013). Examining the relation of religion and spirituality to subjective well-being across national cultures. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality, 5(4), 304-315.
  11. Peng, K., & Nisbett, R. E. (1999). Culture, dialectics, and reasoning about contradiction. American Psychologist54(9), 741-754.
  12. Sue, D., & Sue, S. (1987). Cultural factors in the clinical assessment of Asian Americans. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 479-487.
  13. Sundararajan, L. (2014). The function of negative emotions in the Confucian tradition. In W. G. Parrott (Ed.), The positive side of negative emotions (pp.179-197). New York: Guilford.
  14. Triandis, H. C. (1972). The analysis of subjective culture. New York, NY: John Wiley.
  15. Wong, P. T. P. (1993). Effective management of life stress: The resource-congruence model. Stress Medicine, 9, 51-60.
  16. Wong, P. T. P. (2009). Viktor Frankl: Prophet of hope for the 21st century. In A. Batthyany & J. Levinson (Eds.), Existential psychotherapy of meaning: Handbook of logotherapy and existential analysis. Phoenix, AZ: Zeig, Tucker & Theisen, Inc. An earlier version of this chapter is available at http://www.meaning.ca/archives/archive/art_frankl_prophet_P_Wong.htm 
  17. Wong, P. T. P. (2010). 快乐人生的钥匙 – 正向心理的启迪. In 清朗人生: 全人生命教育論文集, (pp. 65-71): Santou University Press.
  18. Wong, P. T. P. (2011). Positive psychology 2.0: Towards a balanced interactive model of the good life. Canadian Psychology, 52(2), 69-81.
  19. Wong, P. T. P. (2012). Toward a dual-systems model of what makes life worth living. In P. T. P. Wong (Ed.), The human quest for meaning: Theories, research, and applications (2nd ed., pp. 3-22). New York, NY: Routledge.
  20. Wong, P. T. P. (2013). Cross-cultural positive psychology. In K. Keith (Ed.) Encyclopedia of Cross-cultural Psychology. Oxford, UK: Wiley Blackwell Publishers.
  21. Wong, P. T. P., & Reker, G. T. (1985). Stress, coping, and well-being in Anglo and Chinese elderly. Canadian Journal on Aging, 4(1), 29-37.
  22. Wong, P. T. P., & Reker, G. T. (1985). Stress, coping, and well-being in Anglo and Chinese elderly. Canadian Journal on Aging, 4(1), 29-37.
  23. Wong, P. T. P., Reker, G. T., & Gesser, G. (1994). Death Attitude Profile – Revised: A multidimensional measure of attitudes toward death. In R. A. Neimeyer (Ed.), Death anxiety handbook: Research, instrumentation, and application (pp. 121-148). Washington, DC: Taylor & Francis.
  24. Wong, P. T. P., & Sproule, C. F. (1984). Attributional analysis of locus of control and the Trent Attribution Profile (TAP). In H. M. Lefcourt (Ed.), Research with the locus of control construct, Vol. 3: Limitations and extensions (pp. 309-360). New York, NY: Academic Press.
  25. Wong, P. T. P., & Ujimoto, K. V. (1998). The elderly Asian Americans: Their stress, coping, and well-being. In L. C. Lee & N. W. S. Zane (Eds.), Handbook of Asian American Psychology (pp. 165-209). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
  26. Yang, J. (2014). The happiness of the marginalized: Affect, counseling and
    self-reflexivity in China. In J. Yang (ed.), The political economy of affect and emotion in East Asia (pp. 45-62). New York, NY: Routledge.
  27. Yang, K. S. (1988). Chinese filial piety: A conceptual analysis. In K. S. Yang (Ed.), The Chinese mind (pp. 39-73). Taipei, Taiwan: Guigan Book Co. (In Chinese)
  28. Yang, K. S., & Bond, M. H. (1990). Exploring implicit personality theories with indigenous or imported constructs: The Chinese case. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 1087-1095.
  29. Zane, N., & Sue, S. (1986). Ethnic minority issues: Divergent reasoning and diverse solution. In E. Seidman & J. Rappaport (Eds.), Redefining social issues (pp. 289-304). New York, NY: Praeger.
  30. Zevnik, L. (2014). Critical perspectives in happiness research. New York, NY: Springer International Publishing.

Cite

Wong, P. T. P. (2009). Chinese positive psychology. In S. J. Lopez (Ed.), Encyclopedia of positive psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 148-156). Oxford, UK: Wiley Blackwell.